Beliefs about
Drinking Problem Causation
by
Susan
Bullers
University
of North Carolina Wilmington
and
Carol A. Prescott
University
of
Southern California
Background
Individuals' perceptions about the causes
of behavioral and health problems can have
an impact on whether or not they
participate in treatment programs as well
as on their beliefs about the efficacy of
various treatment methods (McKirnan 1984;
Harris and Fennell 1988). In a study
of college students in Chile, Didier
(2001) found that respondents believed the
most effective treatments for drinking
problems would be those corresponding most
directly to their causes. However,
Harris and Fennell (1988) found that
problem drinkers who attributed their
heavy drinking to workplace stress were
less likely to participate in an Employee
Assistance Program than those who had
other attributions for their drinking.
The Health Belief
Model, originally developed by Irwin
Rosenstock, uses psycho-social attitudes
about one's health to understand and
predict health behavior and treatment
compliance. This model was further
refined (Rosenstock et al. 1988) to
include a component of self-efficacy;
individuals who believe that they have
some control over the course or outcome of
a health problem will be more likely to
seek treatment. McKirnan (1984)
agrees that beliefs about the causes of
any abnormal behavior are important to the
course and treatment of the problem and
specifically argues that help-seeking
behavior requires at least some degree of
belief in personal efficacy.
Cultural beliefs
about drinking problem causation have
changed over time; away from moral
weakness explanations toward social
influence or disease model beliefs. For
example, in 1977 Hoy found that causal
beliefs were consistent with a personality
disorder concept, yet in 1984, Furnham
found social influences such as friends,
family and media were the dominant causal
beliefs. Sigelman et al. (1992)
studied a sample of students (6th grade
through college) and found that disease
model explanations constituted the most
common causal belief explanations for
problem drinking but that the majority of
respondents also believed that curing
drinking problems was a personal
responsibility. In 1978, Fisher
discussed the limitations of the emerging
disease model trends for alcohol and in
1994, Cunningham et al. suggested that
there was still conflict about these
causal beliefs. Schomeru et al.
(2011) recently reviewed population
studies regarding the stigma of alcohol
dependence and found that alcoholics were
significantly more likely to be held
responsible for their conditions than were
those with other mental disorders, such as
depression or schizophrenia.
One distinction made
in causal attribution research is whether
causes are perceived to be "internal" or
"external." Internal causation
reflects a belief in which individuals
have some degree of control over their
drinking and are at least partially
responsible for their own drinking
problems. Internal attributions
include factors such as personal
decisions, morality, judgment, and choice
of friends. In contrast, external
causal beliefs attribute drinking problems
to factors beyond the individual's
control. External attributions
include factors such as exposure to
stressors, family upbringing, genetic or
biological predisposition, and bad luck.
Demographic
correlates
Several studies have looked for
demographic correlates of causal
beliefs. Although a long history of
research has found correlations between
generalized internal/external
attributional styles and gender, age,
cohort, income, health and education
(Pearlin and Schooler 1978; Kohn, 1989;
Mirowsky and Ross1989; Bullers and
Prescott 2001), alcohol-specific
attributions may not be associated with
generalized attributional styles. In
preliminary work using a subset of the
current dataset (Bullers and Prescott
2000) failed to find a significant
association between a generalized
perceived control construct and any of
several internal and external problem
drinking causal beliefs in a sample of
college students.
Some studies have found associations
between demographic characteristics and
drinking-specific causation beliefs.
McKirnan (1984) found that higher SES
individuals tend to believe in more
internal (personal responsibility) causes
whereas lower SES individuals were more
likely to attribute alcohol abuse to
external causes (environment, stressful
situations). Genetic and/or biological
factors were not included in this
study. Kauffman et al. (1997) did
include genetic and disease model
variables and found that endorsement of
biological attributions was higher among
individuals with higher levels of
employment and income.
Findings regarding gender differences in
drinking causation beliefs are
contradictory; in 1988 Harris and Fennell
found that females were more likely than
males to attribute alcohol problems to
moral weakness and emotional and
interpersonal problems but in 1997,
Kauffman et al. found that females were
more likely than males to make external
(in this case biological or environmental)
attributions. More recently, Lambert et
al. (2006) found males more likely than
females to make internal (character)
attributions.
In addition to
cultural shifts in beliefs about drinking
problems, cross-sectional studies suggest
there are age effects. Lambert et al.
(2006) found that disease model
attributions were higher and "character"
attributions lower with older age among a
sample of college students. Sigelman
et al. (1992) also found that attributions
to "moral weakness" were lower among older
respondents but found no age differences
for any of the other causes, including
disease, bad environment, normative
motives, or personal responsibility.
However, in separate analyses about curing
drinking problems they found that beliefs
about personal responsibility were higher
with older age.
Few studies have
looked at the role of ethnicity in beliefs
about drinking problem causation.
Aggregate findings have shown that Native
Americans drink more than Whites, who in
turn, drink more than African
Americans. Although there is a vast
literature on ethnicity and alcohol use,
there are also several critiques of this
literature arguing that the relationship
between drinking and ethnicity is fraught
with methodological problems including
confounding socioeconomic factors and lack
of cultural homogeneity among common
ethnic groupings (Caetano et al. 1998;
Fisher 1987). Of studies that have
looked at ethnicity and attributions
regarding problem drinking, Lambert et al.
(2006) found that whites were more likely
to attribute substance abuse problems to
internal (character) causes, whereas
non-whites were more likely to make
disease model attributions. These
results may reflect the disproportionate
representation of non-whites in lower SES
ranges; findings regarding the association
between generalized external attributions
and lower SES are fairly consistent
(Pearlin and Schooler 1978; Kohn 1989;
Mirowsky and Ross 1989; Bullers and
Prescott 2001).
Religion may also
affect drinking problem causation
beliefs. Some religious
denominations explicitly prohibit or
object to alcohol consumption. The
degree of identification with one of these
denominations could have a strong effect
on one's causal beliefs. Viewing
alcohol consumption as a "sin" constitutes
an internal attribution, suggesting that
an individual has control over the course
and outcome of the problem.
Religions in which doctrine explicitly
prohibits alcohol use are referred to as
"objector religions." The
association between membership in objector
Religions and lower drinking rates may
also account for some ethnicity effects:
African Americans have the highest rates
of membership in objector religions in the
US (Kosmin and Keysar 2009).
Relation of
drinking experience to causal beliefs
Personal drinking history and exposure to
others with drinking problems may also
affect beliefs about the causes of alcohol
problems. Research has shown that
heavy drinkers "normalize" their drinking
problem beliefs to exclude themselves from
problem drinking categories (Furnham and
Lowick 1984; Oei et al. 1990; Didier 2001;
Wild 2002). Because heavy drinkers
define problem drinking at much higher
levels of consumption than do light or
non-drinkers, the relationship between
heavy drinking and attribution may be more
complex than a simple linear
association. McKirnan (1984) found
an interaction effect in which heavy
drinking was associated with more internal
attributions among higher SES individuals
but was associated with more external
attributions among lower SES individuals.
In essence, the internal/external
attribution patterns of SES were
accentuated as drinking increased.
In summary, previous
research has found that individuals'
beliefs about the causes of drinking
problems can affect treatment-seeking and
beliefs about the efficacy of
treatments. Some SES, gender,
ethnicity, and age correlates of alcohol
abuse causation beliefs have been observed
in the literature, but the gender findings
are inconsistent and most studies have not
included genetic or disease model causal
options. In addition, few of these
studies considered the respondents'
personal experiences with heavy drinking.
We hypothesized that one's own drinking
and one's exposure to others' problem
drinking could influence causation
beliefs.
Using data from a
sample of 170 University students
(over-sampled for ethnic and age
diversity) we examine the strength of
attributions for ten possible causes
of drinking problems: personal
decision, drinking friends, bad judgment,
weak morals, family upbringing,
stress/life circumstances, genetics,
physiology, bad luck and low
intelligence. A factor analysis of
cause beliefs is conducted to test if
these causes represent distinct causal
dimensions that can be further analyzed as
composite variables. Finally, using OLS
multiple regression we explore the
independent effects of demographic and
drinking experience variables on each
causal factor.
Methods
Sample
This analysis consists of data from two
different survey samples. The first data
set was originally collected to explore
the issues concerning perceived control
and drinking. The second survey was
intended to explore ethnic differences in
perceptions of drinking problem
causation. Because these data sets
both included information concerning
perceptions of drinking problem causation,
they were combined for this study. This
pooling of data sets created a sample with
an adequate numbers of cases in all three
ethnic groups of interest. However,
because these two data sets had
inconsistent measures of some variables,
these variables had to be re-constructed
in a manner that would allow for
consistent and valid measures across both
data sets.
The first data set was collected in 2000
and consisted of 91 respondents from a
large Southern university in a
metropolitan area. Respondents
filled out a seven-page questionnaire in
non-randomly selected university classes.
Purposive class selection included day and
evening extension programs to increase the
age diversity. A purposive range of
subject areas was sampled to avoid
over-reliance on the social sciences. The
sample had mean age of 36.3 (SD=18.4) and
was 63% female. The second data set
was collected in 2003 and included 79
students at a traditionally Native
American University located in a smaller
Southern city. These respondents filled
out questionnaires in non-randomly
selected sociology daytime classes. The
age of this sample was M=27.3 (SD=10.0)
and the sample was 69% female. Six
respondents did not answer the religion
items and five respondents did not answer
the race item reducing the sample size for
this study to 159. An additional
seven respondents answered the race item
with responses other than white, Native
American or African American. The
small number in these ethnic categories
did not allow for meaningful analysis so
these cases were also dropped resulting in
a final sample size of 152. Missing
values in the variables used to construct
the causation factors further reduce
sample sizes in some analyses and will be
noted where appropriate.
Variables
Age is measured in years. Since all
respondents were undergraduate students,
education level did not offer enough
variability for valid inclusion in the
analysis. Household income was also
a problematic indicator of socioeconomic
status (SES) for this population because
about half of the respondents are young
college students living independently
whose household income is not likely to
represent their social class.
Although income and education of family of
origin may be better indicators of social
class for the younger students in this
sample, we expressly sampled a substantial
number of non-traditional students. Income
or education of family of origin for the
older students is equally unlikely to
represent current SES for these
respondents so was not included in
analyses. In addition, the sample is
somewhat homogenous regarding
socioeconomic status, simply by virtue of
being college students.
Ethnicity for this
analysis was categorized as: White,
African American, and Native
American. Other ethnic categories
included too few respondents (6) for
meaningful analysis and were removed from
the sample to avoid un-interpretable
results. Ethnic identification in
this community is complicated because the
Native American community has struggled
for full official tribal recognition for
decades. Although at the time of data
collection for this study full recognition
had not been passed, the latest effort in
2009 included a bill for recognition that
was approved by the United States Senate
Committee on Indian Affairs. In addition
to official tribal recognition, a large
number of residents have both Native
American and African American heritage and
their primary identity may be either
African American or Native American.
Although ethnic categorization is
problematic under normal circumstances, it
is especially difficult here, where ethnic
self-identity may differ from socially and
legally determined ethnicity. We use
a self-defined measure of ethnicity here.
The religion items
differed for the two samples; the urban
campus was asked only for broad religious
category membership with a write-in
denomination option. Many of the write-in
denomination responses did not actually
represent recognized religious
denominations; many used broad categories
such as "Christian," resulting in an
unusable denomination variable for the
urban campus. The item was re-worded into
a separate variable for the rural campus
sample; asking whether their religious
beliefs prohibited alcohol
consumption. One religion variable
was common to both samples and included
the following categories; Protestant,
Catholic, Jewish, other, and none.
Sixty-five percent of the sample were
Protestant, 12.2% were Catholic (not an
"objector" religion), and 21.3% were
"other"; which likely included those who
did not identify with any religion.
Although this measure is not ideal,
"objector" Christian denominations do
constitute the largest of any religious
affiliation category in this "bible belt"
state (Kosmin and Keysar, 2009).
The "causes of
drinking problems" questions asked
respondents: "How much influence do you
think each of the following has in causing
drinking problems?" Response options
were: 0="no influence", 1="weak
influence", 2="strong influence" and
3="complete influence". The causes
included: family upbringing, personal
physiology/chemistry, friends with
drinking problems, bad judgment, low
intelligence, genetic/inherited causes,
personal decisions, weak moral character,
bad luck, and life circumstances/stress.
We also asked an item about advertising
and cultural messages, but this was
inadvertently formatted differently on the
surveys administered to the two different
samples. In preliminary analyses it was
not found to have equivalent measurement
properties in the two groups and so was
eliminated from further analysis.
For own drinking we
used a variable constructed from items
that asked different questions in each
sample. The older, more urban campus
sample was asked, "During the time in your
life when you used alcohol the most, how
much would you usually drink?"
Response options were: never used alcohol,
1-2 drinks, 3-4 drinks, 5-6 drinks 7-9
drinks, and 10 or more drinks. This scale
was re-coded to mid-points resulting in
scores of 0, 1.2, 3.5, 5.6, 8, and
10. For the younger rural sample,
respondents were asked, "How many drinks
do you typically have on an
occasion?" Although this
inconsistency is problematic, traditional
college age is typically the life stage at
which people drink the most.
Responses to this item for both younger
and older respondents are likely to
reflect their heaviest drinking
experiences so far. As with most
alcohol measures, this variable produced a
non-normal distribution with over 20%
reporting "never used alcohol." We
re-coded the consumption data to reflect
non-drinkers, light/moderate drinkers and
heavy drinkers. Heavy drinking was
defined as four or more drinks per
drinking occasion. The recoded
drinking scale for older urban sample
placed those above the 3-4 drinks category
into the heavy drinking category. There
are several ways to measure and define
heavy drinking but this cutoff point
corresponds roughly to the National
Institute of Alcohol Abuse and
Alcoholism's (NIAAA) definition of problem
drinking (NIAAA, 2009).
Exposure to any
close others' drinking problems was
ascertained by asking, "Have you ever
lived with anyone who had an alcohol
problem?" This item will encompass
the residents of any households that the
respondent has lived in, including
parents, siblings, other relatives,
roommates, partners, spouses, or children.
We were attempting to capture beliefs
arising from daily interactions with and
observations of the problem drinker,
rather than asking about family history of
alcoholism which may have occurred at a
time other than when the respondent was in
residence.
Analysis
First, sample means, standard deviations
and modes for individual cause beliefs and
Cause Factors will be calculated and
presented to examine the rank ordering of
the individual causal beliefs.
Internal/external distinctions will be
indicated. Next, sample descriptives
will be computed giving frequencies and
percentages for all demographic, drinking,
and drinking exposure groups.
We hypothesized that
we would find evidence for three factors,
including a factor of personal
characteristics identified by bad
judgment, personal decisions, weak moral
character, and to a lesser extent, low
intelligence; and a second factor of
external influences, including family
upbringing, life circumstances/stress,
friends with drinking problems, and bad
luck. We hypothesized that the two
biological causes, genetics and
physiology, would form a separate
factor. We tested this structure
using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)
and indexed fit using RMSEA, with "good
fit" represented by e<.08 (e.g., Kline,
2011). We also compare the fit of the CFA
models to the best possible fit from
3-factor and 4-factor exploratory models.
Finally, using OLS
regression analysis, each of the cause
factors will be regressed on the
demographic, drinking, and alcohol
exposure variables. The factor analyses
were conducted using the Mplus structural
modeling program (version 5.0, Muthen
& Muthen, 2005). All other analyses
were conducted using SPSS version 17.
Results
Descriptive statistics (Table 1) show
that 64.5% of the sample was female,
76.3% were white; 11.8% were African
American, and another 11.8% were Native
American. Protestants comprised 69.1% of
the sample. Over-sampling of older
students resulted in a relatively even
distribution of students in the four age
categories; 18-21, 22-25, 26-39, and
40+. Respondent drinking status
was distributed as 20.4% non-drinkers,
45.4% light/moderate drinkers, and 34.2%
heavy drinkers. These are
relatively low rates for college
students and are likely affected by the
over-sampling of older and minority
students. Over one-third (36.2%) of the
respondents reported having lived with a
problem drinker.
Table 1: Sample
Descriptives by Cause Factor Means
|
N
|
%
|
Personal
Cause
|
Biological
Cause
|
Social
Cause
|
Age 18-21
|
44
|
28.9
|
1.56
|
1.27
|
1.71
|
Age 22-25
|
38
|
25.0
|
1.43
|
1.38
|
1.86
|
Age 26-39
|
32
|
21.1
|
1.70
|
1.75
|
2.04
|
Age 26-39
|
32
|
21.1
|
1.70
|
1.75
|
2.04
|
Age 40+
|
38
|
25.0
|
1.43
|
1.79
|
1.67
|
Gender: Female
|
98
|
64.5
|
1.50
|
1.52
|
1.84
|
Gender: Male
|
54
|
35.5
|
1.58
|
1.54
|
1.76
|
Non-Drinker
|
31
|
20.4
|
1.75
|
1.42
|
2.02
|
Light Drinker
|
69
|
45.4
|
1.56
|
1.65
|
1.84
|
Heavy Drinker
|
52
|
34.2
|
1.35
|
1.43
|
1.65
|
Lived with Problem
Drinker
|
55
|
36.2
|
1.58
|
1.66
|
1.88
|
Did Not Live with
Problem Drinker
|
97
|
63.8
|
1.49
|
1.45
|
1.78
|
Protestant Faith
|
105
|
69.1
|
1.58
|
1.54
|
1.88
|
Other Faith
|
47
|
30.9
|
1.39
|
1.53
|
1.64
|
White
|
116
|
76.3
|
1.44
|
1.62
|
1.72
|
African American
|
18
|
11.8
|
1.74
|
1.37
|
2.09
|
Native American
|
18
|
11.8
|
1.82
|
1.17
|
2.04
|
Total Mean
|
|
|
1.53
|
1.53
|
1.82
|
N(a)
|
152
|
100
|
141
|
143
|
144
|
(a)Missing values in
the cause factor variables account for
decreases in n's in cause means.
Figure 1 shows the
mean endorsement scores for each of the
ten causes. Personal decisions received
the highest rating as a cause of drinking
problems, followed by family upbringing,
and stress/circumstances. There was very
little support for the low intelligence
cause. There was not a clear pattern
of preference for either internal or
external causes.
The results from our
initial confirmatory factor analysis
indicated that the bad luck item did not
load on the hypothesized factor (with
the other external causes), having a
loading of <.30. An additional
analysis freeing its loading indicated
that it did not load better on the other
factors, so we opted to eliminate it
from the structure and use the remaining
9 causes. The resulting structure is
shown in Figure 2.
The model obtained
c2=48.3 with 24 df, and fit was good:
CFI=.94, RMSEA=.077. This compared
to c2=419 with 36 df for a baseline
model of no association for these 9
items. Although the loadings are not all
high, they are consistent with our
hypothesized structure. As a further
check, we ran an exploratory factor
analysis using the EFA option in Mplus.
This identified the 3-factor solution as
the most parsimonious for these data
(the first four eigenvalues were:
3.01, 1.56, 1.10 and 0.89) and the fit
of a minimally structured three factor
model (with 12 df) was not substantially
better: c2=21.1, i.e., a
difference of 27.2 on 12 df.
Furthermore, examination of the loadings
of the EFA solutions did not suggest the
fit could be improved by moving any
items to a different factor. We thus
conclude that our slightly-revised
hypothesized three factor solution
provides a good summary of the way
participants were responding to these
items. We therefore created weighted
factor scores for each participant by
summing the items loading on the factor
and dividing by the number of items
(i.e., 3 for social, 2 for biology, 4
for personal) so that the scores for
each would be in comparable units.
The group average
scores on the factors are shown with the
causes in Figure 1. Overall, there was
higher endorsement of the social cause
factor than for either the personal or
biological cause factors; which had
similar average scores. Table 1 shows the
mean scores on the three cause factors for
the sample divided by demographic and
drinking variables. The 26-39 age
group gave higher scores to both personal
and social causes than did the other age
groups, whereas the biological cause
scores were higher across age. Both
personal and biological cause attribution
were negatively associated with drinking
level, and Protestants scored social
causes higher than did non-Protestants.
Ethnicity results suggest that whites
differed from both African Americans and
Native Americans, who made similar
attributions on all three cause
factors. Preliminary analyses found
minimal or no differences by gender on
causal beliefs, so this variable was not
included in the regression models.
The results from the regression analyses
are shown in Table 2. Of the three models,
the demographic and alcohol exposure
variables explained the least amount of
variation in the personal cause factor
(Adj. R2= .084), and the most amount of
variation in the social cause factor (Adj.
R2= .144). All three cause factors
were significantly associated with age,
but in different ways: among older
respondents, personal and social causation
beliefs were lower but biological
causation beliefs were higher.
Consistent with the results in Table 1,
Protestants were significantly more likely
to attribute drinking problems to personal
and social causes than were
non-protestants, but there was no
association between religion and
biological causation beliefs. Heavy
drinkers were less likely than non- and
light/moderate drinkers to attribute
drinking problems to personal or social
causes, but the drinking status groups did
not differ in biological cause
beliefs. Those who had lived with a
problem drinker were more likely than
others to attribute drinking problems to
personal and biological causes but did not
differ for social causes.
Table 2:
Regression of Cause Factor Scores on
Demograhic,
Own Drinking and Exposure Variables
|
Personal
Cause
|
Biological
Cause
|
Social
Cause
|
Constant
|
1.590
(.159)
|
1.209
(.188)
|
1.861
(.135)
|
Age in Years
|
-.007
(.003)*
|
.013
(.004)**
|
-.008
(.003)**
|
Native American(a)
|
.244
(.165)
|
-.314
(.190)
|
.194
(.134)
|
African American
|
.168
(.165)
|
-.286
(.194)
|
.176
(.139)
|
Protestant(b)
|
.221
(.109)*
|
-041
(.128)
|
.279
(.092)**
|
Non-Drinker(c)
|
.072
(.120)*
|
-.198
(.160)
|
.097
(.114)
|
Heavy Drinker
|
-.264
(.120)*
|
-.131
(.141)
|
-2.32
(.101)*
|
Lived with Problem
Drinker
|
.080
(.103)*
|
.256
(.121)*
|
.093
(.087)
|
Adjusted Rsquare
|
.084
|
.125
|
.44
|
N
|
141
|
143
|
144
|
(a)Reference category is white.
(b)Reference category is non-Protestant
(c)Reference category is light/moderate
drinker.
*p<.05
**p<.01
Discussion
Despite suggestions in the health
literature that many behavioral problems
have become "medicalized," attributions
among this sample favored social and
personal rather than biological
explanations. The strength of
endorsement of causal beliefs about
problem alcohol use shows no clear
preference for either internal or external
causes. The factor analysis
supported the existence of three factors.
A personal cause factor is generally
consistent with internal causation
beliefs; bad judgment, personal decisions,
and morality are generally seen as
characteristics over which individuals do
have control. However, the low
intelligence component is somewhat
ambiguous. It can hold an internal
meaning similar to bad judgment if viewed
as a situational state, or it can reflect
an external attribution, similar to that
of genetics or physiology. In either
case, support for this particular cause
component was consistently low.
A Biological
cause factor comprised of genetics and
physiology, most clearly reflects
deterministic external attributions. It
received relatively low support overall,
but was more strongly endorsed by older
participants. Whether this is an age
or cohort effect is unclear, but it seems
inconsistent with increasing societal
trends toward medical explanations.
The social cause
factor presents an interesting case with
respect to internal/external
attributions. Although family and
stress are generally not under one's
control, they are not as deterministic as
biological causes. Although social
factors may lead to problem drinking, it
is generally believed that one can
overcome detrimental social influences and
learn to cope with stress in a
non-destructive manner. This factor then,
provides an attribution that is neither
distinctly internal nor distinctly
external. The implications of this
attribution for treatment-seeking and
treatment efficacy are unclear but it does
suggest some level of individual agency.
Consistent with the
literature, heavier drinkers have lower
endorsement of personal and social
attributions of drinking problems.
Individuals who had lived with a problem
drinker were more likely than those who
had not to attribute drinking problems to
personal and biological causes. This
effect may be associated with the
first-hand, day-to-day experiences of
observing the struggles and dealing with
the frustrations of someone who is
addicted to alcohol.
Protestants were
more likely than non-protestants to
attribute drinking problems to personal
and social causes. Although specific
denomination data were not available for
this analysis, these findings may reflect
the effects of "objector" religious
doctrine which is found predominantly in
protestant denominations and is especially
prevalent in the U.S. South.
Interpreting drinking problems as a breach
in piety or as a deviation from religious
doctrine would account for the
attributions to both personal and social
cause beliefs. These religious
interpretations suggest that drinking
problems can, and should, be addressed and
overcome.
There were no
significant ethnicity effects. As
discussed earlier; categorizing ethnic
identity in this community is complicated.
Overlap between African American and
Native American social identities may
result in similar effects among the two
groups. However, post-hoc analyses
using a combined "ethnic minority"
category also failed to detect any
significant ethnic differences versus
Whites. As noted earlier, the effects of
ethnicity on internal/external
attributions in the literature may have
been due to unmeasured SES effects;
specifically education. The
homogeneity of educational attainment in
this sample may have controlled for these
effects to a great extent.
Conclusions
This study found that older respondents
were more likely than younger respondents
to attribute drinking problems to
biological causes and were less likely to
attribute to personal causes or social
causes. Heavy drinkers were
less likely than others to attribute to
internal causes, suggesting they may also
be at increased risk regarding treatment
access. Protestants as opposed to
non-Protestants, showed a pattern of
internal attribution, suggesting beliefs
about personal efficacy in controlling
drinking.
The emergence and
support of the social cause factor
suggests that current drinking problem
causation beliefs may not reflect a clear
an internal/external dichotomy. The
social cause belief suggests that external
factors, such as family or stress, may
cause drinking problems but these causes
are not insurmountable or deterministic.
The effect of this attribution on
treatment efficacy is unknown, but it does
suggest an element of individual
agency. Protestants were more likely
to attribute drinking problems to the
social cause factor, whereas older
respondents and heavy drinkers were less
likely to attribute to social cause
factors.
Although the
literature suggests that some internal
attribution is necessary for effective
treatment outcomes, over-reliance on
personal attribution may also hamper
treatment efforts. Blaming all
treatment setbacks or difficulties on
personal shortcomings could negatively
impact motivation in treatment efforts.
Knowledge of the biological mechanisms
involved may inform the treatment process
and balance self-blaming and reproach. In
addition, beliefs about the genetic risk
for alcoholism often prevent or limit
drinking for at-risk individuals.
Limitations of this
study include a non-random sample and
inconsistent measurement of religious
denomination and current drinking across
the two samples. Because these data were
originally collected to address other
issues compromises had to made in the
construction of some of these meaures in
order realize the benefits of the greater
ethnic and age variation and a larger
sample size in the combined data. In
addition, the difficulties inherent in
measuring social class among college
students also prohibited a rigorous SES
measure. Future data collection
could easily overcome these measurement
issues and could also allow for an
analysis of changes in these attitudes
over time. Ideally, future research
would look at these drinking problem
attributions in a representative
population with more educational and SES
variability. Finally, findings from
this line of inquiry can be used to
examine the effects of these causal
beliefs on treatment-seeking and treatment
efficacy.
References
Bullers,
Susan and Carol A. Prescott. 2001. "The
Independent Contributions of Genetics,
Shared Family Environment, and Adult Roles
to Adult Perceived Control." Sociological
Inquiry 71(2):145-163.
Bullers, Susan and Carol A. Prescott.
2000. "Beliefs about the Causes of
Drinking Problems, Exposure to Drinking
Problems and Perceived Control." American
Journal of Medical Genetics,
Special Issue: Abstracts of
Presentations: Eighth World Congress on
Psychiatric Genetics, Versailles,
France, 7 August 2000, 96(4): 451–572.
Caetano, Raul, Catherine L. Clark, and
Tammy Tam. 1998. "Alcohol
Consumption Among Racial/Ethnic
Minorities." Alcohol Health and
Resarch World 22(4):233-238.
Cunningham, John A., Linda Sobell,
Jonathan L. Freedman, and Mark B.
Sobell. 1994. "Beliefs About the Causes
of Substance Abuse: A Comparison or
Three Drugs." Journal of Substance
Abuse 6:219-26.
Didier, Marcelo. 2001. "Attributions in
the Evaluation of Alternative Solutions
to Social Problems." The Journal of
Social Psychology 127 (4): 357-65.
Fisher, Jonathan A.D. 1987. "Alcoholism
and Race; The Misapplication of Both
Concepts to North American Indians."
Canadian Review of Sociology and
Anthropology 24:81-98.
Ford, Julie and Charles Kadushin. 2002.
"Between Sacral Belief and Moral
Community: A Multidimensional Approach
to the Relationship Between Religion and
Alcohol Among Whites and Blacks." Sociological
Forum 17(2):255-279.
Furnham, Adrian, and Victoria Lowick.
1984. "Attitudes to Alcohol Consumption:
The Attribution of Addiction". Social
Science and Medicine 18(8):673-681.
Harris, Michael M., and Mary L. Fennell.
1988. "Perceptions of an Employee
Assistance Program and Employees'
Willingness to Participate." The
Journal of Applied Behavioral
Science 24(4): 423-38.
Hoy, Richard M. 1977. "Some Findings
Concerning beliefs About Alcoholism." British
Journal of Medical Psychology
50: 227-235.
Kaufman, Stephen E., Paula Silver, and
John Poulin. 1997. "Gender Differences
in Attitudes toward Alcohol, Tobacco,
and Other Drugs." Social Work
42(3):231-41.
Kline, Rex B. (2011). Principles and
Practice of Structural Equation
Modeling (3rd Ed). New York:
Guilford Press.
Kohn, Melvin L. 1989. "Social Structure
and Personality." A Quintessentially
Sociological Approach to Social
Psychology." Social Forces
68(1):26-33.
Kosmin, Barry A. and Keysar Ariela.
2009. "American Religious Identification
Survey. (ARIS) 2008".
http://b27.cc.trincoll.edu/weblogs/AmericanReligionSurvey-ARIS/reports/ARIS_Report_2008.pdf.
Hartford, Connecticut, USA:
Trinity College. Retrieved
2011-08-05.
Lambert, Eric G., Lois A. Ventura, David
N. Baker, and Morris Jenkins.
2006. "Drug Views: Does Race
Matter ?" Journal of Ethnicity
in Criminal Justice 4:93-111.
McKirnan, David J. 1984. "The
Identification of Alcohol Problems:
Socioeconomic Status Differences in
Social Norms and Causal Attributions." American
Journal of Community Psychology
12(4):465-84.
Mirowsky, John, and Catherine E. Ross.
1989. Social Causes of Psychological
Distress. Aldine de Gruyter, New
York.
Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén.
2005. Mplus, The Comprehensive
Modeling Program for Applied Researchers,
User's guide. Los Angeles, CA:
Muthén & Muthén
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and
Alcoholism (NIAAA), "How To Screen For
Heavy
Drinking"http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/
publications/Practitioner/PocketGuide/
pocket_guide5.htm Accessed October 12,
2009
Oei, Tian Po, D. Hokin, and Ross McD.
Young. 1990. "Differences Between
Personal and General Alcohol-Related
Beliefs." The International Journal
of the Addictions
25(6):641-651.
Pearlin, Leonard. I., and Carmi
Schooler. 1978. "The Structure of
Coping." Journal of Health and
Social Behavior 19:2-21.
Rosenstock, Irwin, Victor Strecher, and
Marchall Becker. 1988. "Social Learning
Theory and the Health Belief
Model." Health Education &
Behavior 2(15): 175-183.
Schomerus, Georg, Michael Lucht, Anita
Holzinger, Herbert Matschinger, Mauro G.
Carta, Mathias C. Angermeyer.
2011. "The Stigma of Alcohol
Dependence Compared with Other Mental
Disorders: A Review of Population
Studies". Alcohol 46: 105-112.
Sigelman, Carol K., Stacy Gurstell, and
Amanda K. Stewart. 1992. "The
Development of Lay Theories of Problem
Drinking: Causes and Cures." Journal
of Adolescent Research.
7(3):292-312.
Wild, Cameron T. 2002. "Personal
Drinking and Sociocultural Drinking
Norms: A Representative Population
Study." Journal of Studies on
Alcohol 63(4):469-475.
©2013
by Sociation Today