Political
Ideological Distance between Sociology
Students and their Instructors:
The Effects of Students’
Perceptions
by
Jeremiah B. Wills
Queens University of Charlotte
Zachary W. Brewster
Wayne State University
Jonathan R. Brauer
University of Nebraska-Omaha
Bradley Ray
Indiana University – Purdue
University Indianapolis
Perceptions
surrounding the political nature of the
academy (i.e., liberalism) remain a
salient topic for many academics and
political commentators (Horowitz 2004,
2007; Mariani and Hewitt 2008; Tierney
2011). The ubiquitous liberalism
of professors is virtually uncontested
(Mariani and Hewitt 2008; Zipp and
Fenwick 2006), and this liberalism
stands in contrast to the political
ideologies of the American population in
general. Fosse and Gross (2010), using
pooled 1974-2008 General Social Survey
data, found that on a seven-point
political ideology scale, ranging from
extremely conservative to extremely
liberal, professors averaged 4.45, while
non-professors averaged 3.89.
Although this statistically significant
mismatch between academics and
non-academics might seem small, consider
that this gap is actually greater than
the ideological distances observed by
race, income, and gender. Not
surprisingly, therefore, the causes and
consequences of such incongruent
political ideologies between professors
and the general public is the focus of a
considerable amount of discourse and
scholarship.
Some social
scientists have especially focused their
efforts on exploring professors'
liberalism and the potential
marginalization of conservative faculty
(Fosse and Gross 2010; Rothman, Lichter,
and Nevitte 2005). A recent
article published in Sociation Today
exemplifies this approach. In this
study, Bullers et al. (2010) analyzed
data derived from a survey of 226
faculty members at a comprehensive
public university and found that
conservative faculty were not only more
likely than their moderate or liberal
counterparts to perceive a need to
conceal their political beliefs but also
more likely to report that their
political ideologies had negatively
affected their careers. More
recently, Inbar and Lammers (2012) found
that conservative social psychologists
were more likely than their liberal
colleagues to experience a hostile
climate within their field because of
their political beliefs. Furthermore, a
substantial percentage of social
psychologists reported that they were
willing to discriminate against
conservatives when making hiring
decisions and reviewing grant
applications or manuscripts.
Even though concern
over the contagiousness of faculty
liberalism is the focus of most
political commentary on the topic,
considerably less research has focused
on assessing the effects of professors'
political ideologies on student
outcomes. The perception is that
faculty liberalism leads to
indoctrination on college campuses, and
conservative ideas are marginalized as a
liberal agenda is "pushed" onto students
(see Horowitz 2004). Although
anecdotal evidence of such experiences
abounds (e.g., see www.thefire.org and
www.studentsforacademicfreedom.org),
empirical evidence supporting the
liberal indoctrination hypothesis is
limited (see Mariani and Hewitt 2008;
Woessner and Kelly-Woessner 2009; Zipp
and Fenwick 2006). In fact, with a
few notable exceptions (see
Kelly-Woessner and Woessner 2006, 2009;
Woessner and Kelly-Woessner 2009),
little research has been done on how
students' perceptions of their
instructors' political ideologies
influence their college experiences,
especially in the classroom.
Furthermore, although
we have good estimates of the political
ideologies of college professors, we
know next to nothing about the
ideologies of graduate students who are
being increasingly relied upon to teach
their own courses at PhD-granting
institutions (Benjamin 2002; Burmila
2010). This constitutes a salient
area of empirical neglect given that
significant numbers of undergraduates
take courses taught by graduate
students. Moreover, it is in these
teaching experiences that graduate
student instructors—many of whom will be
future professors—develop their
philosophies about teaching. Therefore,
in this research note we attempt to
contribute to (a) the limited
scholarship on the classroom effects of
students' perceptions of their
instructors' political ideologies and
(b) the significant lack of research on
graduate student instructors. We do so
by asking the following research
question: Do sociology students'
perceived ideological distances from
their graduate student instructors
affect students' experiences in and
evaluations of their courses?
Background
Professors are liberal,
and social science professors are
especially liberal—on average (Bullers et
al. 2010; Klein and Stern 2006). In a
survey of over 1200 faculty throughout the
United States, Smith, Mayer, and
Fritschler (2008) found that 58% of
faculty members self-reported as liberal,
while 80% of sociologists did so.
According to Gallup, only 21% of the US
population identifies as liberal (Saad
2012). Probably since Plato's Academy,
academics have been distinguishable from
non-academics in terms of their
worldviews, so a persisting political
mismatch is unsurprising. However, given
the evolution of the modern university
from an independent place of knowledge
production to a site of commercialization
of knowledge and professional career
training (Goldstein 2010), the size of the
ideological gulf between professors and
the rest of Americans is noteworthy.
Furthermore, in pop-culture debates, this
gap is used as evidence of the
marginalization of conservative thinkers
and students in higher education (Horowitz
2004, 2007).
According to
Surber (2010), professors are more liberal
because of their "practical deliberation,
factual investigation, and rational and
moral conviction." Although a complete
explanation for the mismatch is likely
more complex than this (see Fosse and
Gross 2010), Surber's assumptions
contribute to the cultural picture of
professors as superior thinkers who have
carefully found the answers and see it as
their duty to share their "moral
convictions" with students. To the
degree that liberal professors are
exemplified by such a portrayal, it is not
surprising that, relative to their
conservative counterparts, they are more
likely to confess that their ideology
becomes manifested in their teaching
(Bullers et al. 2010). In such a
scenario, it might be expected that the
classroom experiences of students whose
political ideologies are discordant with
their professors may differ from their
counterparts whose political ideologies
are congruent with their professors.
However, as others have pointed out, scant
systematic evaluation of such
marginalization has been conducted
(Kelly-Woessner and Woessner 2006, 2009;
Mariani and Hewitt 2008; Tollini 2009).
Students form
perceptions about their instructors (and
vice versa) —likely before a course ever
begins—and these perceptions are used to
define the classroom experience throughout
the semester and beyond. Certainly
one perception, among many, that appears
to matter within the classroom concerns
the political leanings of instructors
(Dixon and McCabe 2006; Kelly-Woessner and
Woessner 2006, 2009). Although many
students are likely unversed in the
cultural debate about politics in the
academy, and might not even notice biased
teaching when they are exposed to it
(Tollini 2009), they likely do enter the
classroom with a cultural script in mind
for the occupation we call professor. As
Fosse and Gross (2010) suggest, professors
have a certain occupational reputation
that is undeniably liberal. Thus,
conservative students might come to their
college classes with messages they have
received from parents and others warning
of the liberal nature of their
instructors; liberal students, on the
other hand, might be more likely to assume
a shared political philosophy exits
between them and their professors.
Furthermore, given that sociologists are
among the most liberal within the academy
and that sociology as a discipline tends
to involve politically-charged issues, we
thought it important to evaluate how
instructors' and students' political
ideologies influence the sociology
classroom.
To assess
sociology students' perceptions of their
instructors' political ideologies, we
largely follow the work of Kelly-Woessner
and Woessner (2006, 2009; see also
Woessner and Kelly-Woessner 2008). Using a
sample of 1,385 political science
students, they found that greater
perceived partisan and ideological
distance between students and professors
resulted in negative evaluations of
professors and their courses. For
instance, the authors found that the
greater the perceived distance, the more
likely students were to report feeling
that their professors were not open to
discussing diverse viewpoints and that
their instruction lacked
objectivity. Ideological distance
was further found to be predictive of
students' perceptions of their professors'
commitment to student learning and
intellectual growth such that students
whose partisan affiliation was perceived
to be discordant with their professors
were more likely to report that their
professors do not care about students. As
might be expected, the authors also found
that students who perceived their partisan
affiliations to be discordant with their
professors expressed less interest in the
subject, exerted less effort, and were
less likely to recommend the course to
fellow students.
The Kelly-Woessner and
Woessner (2006) study makes clear that
students' perceptions matter, and that a
perceived ideological mismatch potentially
creates an unfavorable climate for student
learning as professors who are seen as
different from their students are likely
discredited and students simultaneously
disengage from the course content.
There is evidence for how perceived
political distance affects political
science students, so how do such
perceptions influence students in
sociology courses taught by graduate
student instructors?
Methods
We administered surveys
toward the end of the semester to a
convenience sample of 277 sociology
students within the classrooms of five
different graduate student instructors at
a large research university.
Completed surveys were obtained from 205
of those students sampled, yielding a
response rate of 74%. Survey
questions were mostly replications of
those used by Kelly-Woessner and Woessner
(2006).
Measures
Our key
independent variable was the perceived
ideological distance between students and
their instructors. Students were
asked their political ideology on a
5-point scale ranging from extremely
liberal (1) to extremely conservative (5),
and their perception of their instructor's
ideology on the same scale. Our
ideological distance variable is the
absolute value of the difference between
student's self-reported political ideology
and the perceived ideology of their
instructor.
We examined the effect
of perceived ideological distance across
eight dependent variables that were
constructed to capture different
dimensions of students' classroom
experiences and evaluations. The
Instructor Evaluation Scale (α = 0.77)
consists of five indicators asking
students to rate (on a 5-point scale) the
degree to which their instructor
objectively presented material, graded
fairly, encouraged students to express
their own views, provided a comfortable
learning environment, and cared about
students and their success. The
Student-Instructor Relationship Scale (α =
0.78) captures the likelihood students
were to talk to their instructor after
class, ask the instructor for a letter of
recommendation, recommend the instructor
to other students, and stay in contact
with the instructor after the semester
terminates. Another dependent
variable we used was students' responses
to a statement about whether they would
consider sociology as a major after taking
their current course, where 5= Strongly
Agree and 1= Strongly Disagree.
For the Silenced
Beliefs Scale (α = 0.63) students were
given Likert-style responses for questions
about whether they wrote what the
instructor wanted to hear rather than
their own opinions on assignments, did not
feel comfortable expressing their own
opinions in the classroom, and felt the
need to hide their own beliefs when
participating or doing coursework.
Students also were asked how often during
the class they felt disinterested, angry,
isolated, and unhappy, and these responses
constitute the Negative Emotions Scale (α
= 0.42). Although each indicator
reflects a separate emotion that may stem
from distinct etiological pathways, all
four indicators of classroom affect are
combined into a single index because each
has been associated with reduced
motivation, self-regulation, and
achievement in academic settings (Pekrun
et al. 2002). Relatedly, the low alpha
coefficient for this index is unsurprising
because internal consistency should not
always be expected when measuring such
multidimensional constructs (Boyle 1991;
Cattell 1978; Hayes, Nelson, and Jarrett
1987). The Discrimination Scale (α =
0.87) asked students how often they felt
discriminated against or singled out in
class because of race, sex, political
beliefs, religious beliefs, physical
appearance, and involvement in
extracurricular activities (athletics,
clubs, etc.). We also constructed a
Disagreement Scale, which consists of
students' responses to two statements
about how often they found themselves
disagreeing with their professor's
viewpoints and the readings or other
course content (r = 0.58 for the two items
used in the scale). Finally, we used
as an outcome variable responses to a
statement asking students if the course
provided useful skills and/or knowledge,
where 5= Strongly Agree and 1= Strongly
Disagree.
In our OLS regression
analyses, we controlled for the effects of
students' age, gender, and expected grade
in the course. We also included dummy
variables to control for instructor
effects. Unfortunately, we did not
have enough variability by instructors or
classrooms in our sample to allow for
adequate examination of classroom-level or
instructor-level contextual effects.
However, we were able to check the
robustness of our OLS regression findings
by replicating all models using a
hierarchical linear modeling approach that
accounted for our clustered data structure
by nesting students within classrooms.
Results of these multilevel models
confirmed conclusions derived from our OLS
analyses.
Results
Students rated their
political ideology on average as neither
liberal nor conservative (M= 3.02; SD=
.87) on a 5-point scale, and they rated
their professor's political ideology as
liberal (M= 2.25; SD= .73). This
yielded an average political distance
score of about 1 (see Table 1).
Table
1: Summary Statistics (N=193)*
Variable
|
Mean or %
|
SD
|
Min.-Max
|
Political
Ideological Distance
|
1.04
|
.97
|
0-4
|
Age
|
19.19
|
2.18
|
17-39
|
Male Sex
|
47.7%
|
|
|
Expected
Course Grade
|
4.24
|
.73
|
2-5
|
Instructor
Evaluation Scale
|
20.84
|
2.65
|
11-25
|
Student-Instructor
Relationship Scale
|
11.87
|
3.38
|
4-20
|
Consider
Majoring in Sociology
|
2.63
|
1.14
|
1-5
|
Silenced
Beliefs Scale
|
6.43
|
1.94
|
3-12
|
Negative
Emotions Scale
|
8.91
|
2.12
|
4-17
|
Discrimination
Scale
|
7.87
|
3.35
|
6-30
|
Disagreement
Scale
|
4.92
|
1.37
|
2-9
|
Useful
Course
|
3.85
|
.92
|
1-5
|
*This sample was generated via. listwise
deletion of missing data.
Table 2 contains the results of our
multivariate OLS regression analyses where
we model the effects of political
ideological distance on eight outcomes
related to students' classroom
experiences. Ideological distance has a
statistically significant negative effect
on instructor evaluations and on the
student-instructor relationship. In
addition, students are less likely to
consider majoring in sociology as
perceived ideological distance
increases. Further, our models
predict that as ideological distance
increases students are more likely to
silence their beliefs and experience
negative emotions. Moreover, with
greater political ideological distance,
students report increased perceived
discrimination and greater frequencies of
disagreement with their professor and
course content. Finally, ideological
distance appears unrelated to whether
students think their current course is
useful. In sum, we find deleterious
effects for political ideological distance
on seven of the eight outcomes related to
students' experiences in their sociology
class.
Table
2. OLS Coefficients Predicting
Students' Classroom Experience
Variable
|
Instructor
Evaluation
Scale
|
Student-
Instructor
Relation
-ship
Scale
|
Consider
Majoring
in Socio-
logy
|
Silenced
Beliefs
Scale
|
Negative
Emotions
Scale
|
Discrim.
Scale
|
Disagree
Scale
|
Useful
Course
|
Intercept
|
16.38*
|
9.86*
|
1.79*
|
8.16*
|
9.21*
|
10.84*
|
5.08*
|
3.04*
|
Ideological
Distance
|
1.10*
|
-.504*
|
-.326*
|
-.442
|
-.523
|
-1.76*
|
.503*
|
.02
|
Ideological
Distance
Squared
|
-.684*
|
|
|
.447*
|
.357*
|
1.04*
|
|
|
Age
|
.138
|
-.024
|
.029
|
-.001
|
.014
|
.015
|
.001
|
-.006
|
Male
|
.238
|
-.874
|
-.098
|
-.263
|
.108
|
-.604
|
.119
|
-.115
|
Expected
Grade
|
.652
|
1.01*
|
.213
|
-.525*
|
-.221
|
-.800*
|
-.192
|
.308*
|
Adj. R
square
|
.246
|
.196
|
.114
|
.153
|
.115
|
.152
|
.124
|
.217
|
n
|
205
|
196
|
206
|
206
|
205
|
203
|
206
|
206
|
*p<.05
Table entries are unstandardized coefficients.
Models also include dummy variables to control
for instructor effects.
A squared term for ideological distance was
explored for each outcome; however, we
report only the trimmed models here.
Notice from Table 2
that we fit a squared term for ideological
distance in our models. A few of the
non-linear effects are worth noting. For
the silenced beliefs and negative emotions
outcomes, the models suggest that some
perceived ideological distance is
inconsequential for students (see the
statistically non-significant first-order
effects). However, the second-order terms
show that, as perceived ideological
distance becomes greater than one, reports
of silenced beliefs and negative emotions
increase. We find statistically
significant first- and second-order terms
for the instructor evaluation and
discrimination scales. With little
perceived ideological distance,
instructors are evaluated more positively
and perceived discrimination is less
likely (see the statistically significant
first-order terms). When perceived
ideological distance becomes greater than
one, however, instructor evaluations
decline and perceived discrimination
increases. These non-linear effects
suggest that a small amount of perceived
ideological distance is inconsequential
for some outcomes (i.e., silenced beliefs
and negative emotions) and interpreted
favorably by students according to other
outcomes (i.e., instructor evaluations and
perceived discrimination). Perhaps some
ideological difference creates interest
and promotes engagement in the classroom.
Alternatively, these findings might
reflect more moderate instructors'
preference to convey course content
objectively without making his/her
political beliefs a salient issue in the
classroom.
Discussion
and Conclusions
A considerable amount
of attention has been given to the nature
of professors' liberal majority on college
campuses, and there is a growing amount of
empirical evidence that reveals negative
consequences of this liberal climate for
conservative professors (Bullers et al.
2010; Inbar and Lammers 2012). Scholarship
on students' experiences with their
professors' political ideologies, however,
is limited, and this is especially the
case with regard to professors-in-training
(i.e., graduate student instructors). The
most relevant exception is the work of
Kelly-Woessner and Woessner (2006), whose
work we extended here using a sample of
sociology undergraduate students who were
instructed by graduate students at a large
research university.
We found that when
students' perceive themselves to be at
ideological odds with their graduate
student instructors their experience in
the classroom is compromised. Even though
students are more likely to perceive these
instructors as liberal than conservative,
our models suggest that students'
perceived distance is important regardless
of the direction of this distance. As the
mismatch between students' political
beliefs and those of their instructors
grows, students report increasing levels
of negative classroom experiences. We
found that students silence their own
beliefs, experience negative emotions, and
perceive that they are discriminated
against, among other outcomes. It is
logical to suspect that these types of
negative classroom experiences have the
potential to compromise student learning
and university engagement. These
experiences, based on students'
perceptions, have real consequences for
instructors, too, in the form of lower
instructor evaluations, diminished
relationships with students, and a reduced
willingness for students to consider
sociology as a major.
The perceived
political ideologies of the graduate
student instructors in our sample are
comparable with other estimates of student
perceptions of professors' ideologies
(Kelly-Woessner and Woessner 2006) and
faculty self-report data (Zipp and Fenwick
2006). Therefore, it is unlikely we
found such consistent negative effects for
ideological distance because graduate
student instructors are perceived as
exceptionally liberal. More research
needs to be done, however, on graduate
student instructors' political ideologies
and how they handle political and
controversial issues in the
classroom. Researchers might even
consider whether perceived ideological
distance from students shapes graduate
students' teaching philosophies and
pedagogies.
Given that most
college and university instructors,
irrespective of rank and discipline, wish
to optimize, not compromise, students'
learning experiences in the classroom, our
findings will understandably invoke
concern. Although we are
uncomfortable suggesting solutions based
on our limited study, we do want to make
two observations. First, we do not think
our findings necessitate a call for
value-free teaching. In sociology classes,
political ideology permeates course
content, discussions, and pedagogy, and
this can be useful as we help students
grapple with and explore important
sociological topics—most all of which are
political in nature. However, our findings
do suggest that instructors need to be
mindful of students' perceptions and
experiences to avoid marginalizing
students if their goal is to construct a
classroom context that maximizes student
learning.
Second, we want
to dissuade professors from thinking it is
their job to close the perceived
ideological distance between them and
their students. Although our data suggest
that ideological homogeneity between
professors and students might improve
course evaluations and some classroom
experiences, we do not believe political
uniformity is a desirable goal. In fact,
ideological homogeneity is antithetical to
higher education, and it would likely
result in undesirable outcomes (e.g.,
weakened critical thinking and dampened
innovation). Rather, we think that the
challenge before us is to develop
approaches for strategically using
political ideological difference to
promote curiosity, engagement, and even
constructive disagreement.
Instructors, regardless of their political
ideologies, need to consider how they can
connect with students with varied
ideological backgrounds and life
experiences.
We have presented
findings derived from a small convenience
sample of undergraduate students at one
large university who were enrolled in one
of five sociology classes, all of which
were taught by graduate students.
Despite the obvious limitations, our study
points to considerable consequences
associated with the issue of
student-instructor political mismatch for
the sociology classroom. Our project
is an attempt to begin filling what is a
sizable gap in knowledge on this issue,
and we urge our colleagues to do more
empirical investigations on students' and
instructors' political ideologies. As a
discipline, we are already far behind the
conclusions on this topic that are held as
common knowledge in popular culture.
References
Benjamin,
Ernst. Fall 2002. "How Over-Reliance on
Contingent Appointments Diminishes
Faculty Involvement in Student Learning." Peer
Review 5(1):1-10.
Boyle, Gregory J.
1991. "Does Item Homogeneity Indicate
Internal Consistency or Item Redundancy in
Psychometric Scales?" Personality and
Individual Differences 12:291-294.
Bullers, Susan,
Melissa Reece, and Christy Skinner. 2010.
"Political Ideology and Perceptions of
Bias Among University
Faculty." Sociation Today 8(2).
Burmila, Edward M.
2010. "Graduate Students as Independent
Instructors: Seven Things to Know about
Teaching Your Own Course while in Graduate
School." PS: Political Science
& Politics 44:557-560.
Cattell, Raymond B.
1978. Personality and Mood by
Questionnaire. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Dixon, Jeffrey C. and
Janice McCabe. 2006. "Competing Perspectives
in the Classroom: The Effect of
Sociology Students' Perceptions of "Balance"
on Evaluations." Teaching
Sociology 34: 111-125.
Fosse, Ethan and Neil
Gross. 2010. "Why are Professors Liberal?"
Unpublished Working Paper.
Goldstein, Harvey.
2010. "The 'Entrepreneurial Turn' and
Regional Economic Development Mission of
Universities." The Annals of Regional
Science 44:83-109.
Hayes, Steven C.,
Rosemary O. Nelson, and Robin B. Jarrett.
1987. "The Treatment Utility of Assessment:
A Functional Approach to Evaluating
Assessment Quality." American
Psychologist 42:963-974.
Horowitz, David. 2004.
"In Defense of Intellectual Diversity." The
Chronicles of Higher Education 50(23):
B12.
Horowitz, David. 2007.
Indoctrination U: The Left's War Against
Academic Freedom. New York:
Encounter Books.
Inbar, Yoel and Joris
Lammers. 2012. "Political Diversity in
Social and Personality Psychology."
Perspectives on
Psychological Science (forthcoming).
Kelly-Woessner, April
and Matthew Woessner. 2006. "My Professor is
a Partisan Hack: How Perceptions of a
Professor's Political Views Affect Student
Course Evaluations." PS: Political
Science & Politics 39:495-501.
Kelly-Woessner, April
and Matthew Woessner. 2008. "Conflict in the
Classroom: Considering the Effects of
Partisan Differences on Political
Education." Journal of Political Science
Education 4:265-285.
Klein, Daniel B. and
Charlotta Stern. 2006. "Sociology and
Classical Liberalism." The Independent
Review XI:37-52.
Mariani, Mack D. and
Gordon J. Hewitt. 2008. " Indoctrination U.?
Faculty Ideology and Changes in
Student Political Orientation." PS:
Political Science and Politics
41:773-783.
Pekrun, Reinhard,
Thomas Goetz, Wolfram Titz, and Raymond P.
Perry. 2002. "Academic Emotions in Students'
Self-Regulated Learning and Achievement: A
Program of Qualitative and Quantitative
Research. Educational Psychologist
37:91-105.
Rothman, Stanley, S.
Robert Lichter, and Neil Nevitte. 2005.
"Politics and Professional Advancement
Among College Faculty." The Forum 3(1):
Article 2. Retrieved January 12, 2010
(http://www.bepress.com/forum/vol3/iss1/art2).
Saad, Lydia. 2012.
"Conservatives Remain the Largest
Ideological Group in U.S." Retrieved
April 29, 2013
(http://www.gallup.com/poll/152021/conservatives-remain-largest-ideological-group.aspx).
Smith, Bruce L. R.,
Jeremy D. Mayer, and A. Lee Fritschler.
2008. Closed Minds: Politics and Ideology in
American Universities. Washington, DC:
Brookings Institution Press.
Surber, Jere P. 2010.
"Well, Naturally We're Liberal." The
Chronicle of Higher Education,
February 7. Retrieved February 10, 2010
(http://chronicle.com/article/Well-Naturally-Were-Liberal/63870).
Tierney, John.
February 7, 2011. "Social Scientist Sees
Bias Within." The New York Times.
Retrieved from
http://www.nytimes.com/2011/02/08/science/08tier.html?_r=0.
Tollini, Craig. 2009.
"The Behaviors that College Students
Classify as Political Bias:
Preliminary Findings and Implications."
Teaching Sociology 37: 379-389.
Woessner, Matthew and
April Kelly-Woessner. 2009. "I Think My
Professor is a Democrat: Considering Whether
Students Recognize and React to Faculty
Politics." PS: Political Science
& Politics 42:343-352.
Zipp, John F and Rudy
Fenwick. 2006. "Is the Academy a Liberal
Hegemony? The Political Orientations
and Educational Values of Professors."
Public Opinion Quarterly 70:304-326.
©2013 by Sociation Today
A Member of the EBSCO Publishing Group
Abstracted in Sociological Abstracts
Online
Indexing and Article Search from the
Directory of Open Access Journals
(DOAJ)
Return to Home Page
|