The Official Journal of the
North Carolina Sociological
Association
A Peer-Reviewed
Refereed Web-Based
Publication
Spring/Summer 2015
Volume 13, Issue 1
An
Exploration of the
Effects of
Work/Family
Roles, Gender-Role
Attitudes and Demographics
on
Normative Drinking
Rates
by
Susan
Bullers
University
of North Carolina Wilmington
Background
The negative impacts of alcohol
abuse on society are well
documented. While the
majority of studies in the
alcohol literature focus on
problem drinking, research
concerning broad societal trends
in normative drinking patterns
can help inform efforts to
predict and target populations
who are at an increased risk for
drinking problems. This study
explores the effects of gender
roles, attitudes and demographic
factors on normative drinking
rates in the US to more clearly
understand how social structural
factors affect alcohol
consumption rates.
Consistent findings in the
literature show that men drink
more, and more often than women
(Dawson, Grant, Chou and
Pickering, 1995; Fillmore et
al., 1997; NIAAA 2009; Wilsnack,
Wilsnack, Kristjanson,
Vogeltanz-Holm, & Gmel,
2009). Although this is
true for virtually all age and
ethnic groups, the gap between
men's and women's rates varies
by age and ethnicity and has
changed over time. Current
trends show that whites have the
highest drinking rates, followed
by Hispanics, with African
Americans reporting the lowest
drinking rates. The gender
gap is widest among Hispanics,
followed by African Americans
and narrowest among
whites. The gender gap is
also wider among older cohorts
and appears to be narrowing over
time. Approaches used to
explain these gender differences
in alcohol use rates include
cultural explanations, which
focus on gendered attitudes and
stigma, and structural
approaches, which focus on
gender roles.
Cultural approaches offer that
alcohol consumption itself is
deeply embedded in a gendered
belief system. Heavy
drinking among women has been
stigmatized and is often
perceived to be associated with
sexual promiscuity, lack of
self-worth, negligence of family
responsibilities, and generally
inappropriate gender-role
behavior (Bergmark, 2004;
Fillmore et al., 1997;
Huselid &Cooper, 1991;
Morgan, 1987; Robbins &
Martin, 1993; Room, 1996;
Trotter, 1985; Waterson,
2000). On the other hand,
heavy drinking has been viewed
as an acceptable way for men to
have fun and gain social
acceptance (Ames & Rebhun,
1996; Gilbert, 1985).
Among young men especially,
heavy drinking can serve as a
culturally recognized symbol of
masculinity (Peralta, 2007;
Peralta, Steele, Nofziger, &
Rickles, 2010). These
traditional attitudes are
diminishing somewhat, especially
among younger cohorts and it is
assumed that increasingly
liberal gender-role attitudes
are responsible for increasing
drinking rates among
women. On the other hand,
the impact of changing gender
role attitudes on men's drinking
has not received as much
attention. In fact, drinking
rates among younger men have
been decreasing. Perhaps
increasingly egalitarian views
on drinking behavior will weaken
the cultural association between
drinking and masculinity,
effectively diminishing the use
of alcohol to bolster masculine
identity.
Although egalitarian gender role
attitudes are associated with
increased drinking among women,
some researchers argue that
social structural factors play a
more fundamental part in this
relationship. The
structural approach to gender
and drinking patterns focuses on
gendered roles themselves and
suggests that increasingly
egalitarian work/family roles
may lead to similar drinking
rates between men and women.
Early stress-related research
assumed that employment roles
were inherently more stressful
than homemaking roles and that
increased employment among women
would be accompanied by
increased stress and subsequent
drinking (Fillmore et al.,
1997). Alternative
explanations pointed out that
women's' family roles were more
likely to be a source of stress
then employment roles (Hammer
& Valgum, 1989; Mirowsky
& Ross, 1989; Wilsnack &
Cheloha, 1987; Wilsnack &
Wilsnack, 1992) and that
home-making roles required
constant attention and
responsibilities that limited
opportunities to drink and
socialize among adults (Ames
& Rebhun, 1996; Caetano
& Clark, 1999; Cunningham,
2007; Waterson, 2000; Wilsnak
& Cheloha, 1987).
The role literature has also
found that marital status
affects drinking; married men
and women drink less than their
single, divorced, or widowed
counterparts, although this
effect is stronger for men than
for women (Fillmore et al.,
1997; Leonard & Rothbard,
1999). Again,
stress-related explanations
argue that non-married
individuals drink to cope with
the hardship and stresses of the
single status.
Alternatives to the stress
explanations argue that heavy
drinkers select out of marriage
and that spouses provide social
support and encourage healthy
behavior; all resulting in lower
drinking rates among married
individuals. Parental
status is also associated with
lower drinking rates, although
more so for women than
men. This is likely due to
gender differences in parental
roles where women typically
spend more time on housework and
child-care than men do (Bianchi,
Milkie, Sayer & Robinson,
2000). However, most
studies in this literature do
not include measures of specific
family roles that may be
implicated in these gender
differences.
The role of college student has
long been associated with very
high drinking rates and a large
body of research has explored
various effects of psychological
and social influences on
drinking rates in this
population (Ham & Hope
2003). From a structural
perspective, the college student
role offers a plethora of
drinking opportunities. College
life typically entails a
flexible schedule, optional
class attendance, few if any
family responsibilities, an
abundance of accommodating and
convenient drinking
establishments and large social
networks. These structural
factors likely play a role in
determining drinking patterns
for this group and combined with
increases in women's college
enrollment, may impact aggregate
drinking rates among
women.
Men have traditionally dominated
roles that accommodate drinking
opportunities (employment and
college) while women's
traditional roles (family and
child-care) limit opportunities
to drink. Increases in
women's' participation in
employment and college roles may
lead to increases in their
drinking rates through the
increased opportunities provided
by these roles. However,
the effects of changing gender
roles on men's drinking have
been largely ignored. In
fact, aggregate trends suggests
that the gender gap convergence
is due to decreases in men's
drinking rather than increases
in women's drinking (McPherson,
Casswell, and Pledger, 2004;
NIAAA, 1994; NIAAA, 2009;
Wallace et al., 2003; Wilsnack,
Wilsnack, Kristjanson,
Vogeltanz-Holm, & Gmel,
2009). The role literature
tends to frame the influence of
changing gender roles as process
in which women's employment will
result in assimilation to male
drinking patterns.
However, increases in men's
participation in family roles
may also be associated with
men's assimilation to women's
drinking patterns.
Increasingly gender-segregated
work and family spheres will
likely result in complex social
influences in which norms will
be determined by the composition
of these new social networks.
Although the role and attitude
factors discussed above are
likely associated with men's and
women's drinking patterns, broad
trends in the US population may
also contribute to aggregate
drinking rates. One factor that
is strongly associated with
decreased drinking rates is age
and the US population is
ageing. This trend could
reduce aggregate drinking rates
(US Bureau of the Census,
2011b). Similarly,
drinking rates are lower in the
US south than other regions and
the US south is growing rapidly
compared to other regions of the
country (US Bureau of the
Census, 2011c).
Another factor that may be
suppressing aggregate drinking
rates is ethnicity. Ethnic
differences in US drinking rates
have a long complex history but
consistent trends show that
whites have the highest rates,
followed by Hispanics and then
African Americans who report the
lowest rates among these three
groups (Bales, 1946; Christmon,
1995; Gilbert, 1991; Gordon,
1985; Herd, 1985; Rebach, 1992;
Collins & McNair,
2004). Relative increases
in minority population sectors
with low drinking rates could
contribute to the overall
suppression of aggregate
drinking rates (US Bureau of the
Census, 2011a).
Another factor shown to affect
drinking rates is "objector"
religious affiliation. Objector
religions include those
religions or denominations that
explicitly prohibit alcohol use.
These religious traditions are
associated with very low
drinking rates and membership
varies substantially by
ethnicity. African
Americans are most likely to
belong to religious
denominations that prohibit
alcohol use, followed by
whites. Hispanics in the
US are more likely to be
Catholic; a denomination not
typically associated with
drinking prohibition. Minority
ethnicity and objector religious
identification are both
associated with low drinking
rates and some of the effects of
ethnicity on drinking and found
in the literature may be due to
these religious
differences. Similarly,
population trends regarding
growth among ethnic groups or
objector religious affiliations
may play a role in aggregate
drinking rate trends.
Given the same drinking
opportunities and expectations,
some people become problem
drinkers and others do
not. The personal,
economic, and social costs of
alcohol abuse in the US are huge
and a vast body of research has
explored the complex
relationships among the social,
psychological, and physiological
factors associated with problem
drinking. Although the process
and causal factors associated
with problem drinking
undoubtedly differ from the
correlates of light or moderate
drinking, it is likely that
structural factors will be
associated with problem drinking
to some extent, simply as a
matter of initial exposure and
opportunity. The extent of
differences between structural
causes of drinking status and
the structural causes of
drinking amount is
unknown.
In summary, changes in gender
role attitudes and work/family
roles have been associated with
increases in women's drinking.
However, the effect of these
factors on men's drinking need
to be examined as well. In
addition, any effects of age,
ethnicity, region, and religion
on drinking are likely to effect
aggregate drinking rates though
the population growth or decline
within these groups. This study
will analyze a large
representative sample to examine
the effects of attitudes, roles,
and demographics on drinking
status and drinking quantity
among men and women.
Methods
Data
This analysis uses data from
Wave II of the US National
Survey of Families and
Households (NSFH) to explore the
effects of demographic,
work/family, gender-role
attitude, religious, and
geographic context variables on
drinking status and drinking
quantity. The NSFH is a
nationally representative
longitudinal panel (three Waves)
survey conducted between 1987
and 2002 (a full description of
data collection methods can be
found in Sweet & Bumpass,
1996). Wave II data were
collected in 1993 and consist of
10,007 respondents. For these
analyses, data were weighted by
a normalized total person weight
for Wave II, which includes a
total person weight from Wave I,
a correction for tracing bias, a
correction for non-response
bias, and a post-stratification
correction. Although Wave
II of the NSFH is somewhat dated
it includes a unique collection
of role, attitude, demographic
and drinking variables not
available elsewhere. These
data are ideal for exploring the
simultaneous and independent
effects of these variables in a
nationally representative
sample. The demographic
effects regarding ethnicity,
region, urban residence, and
age, in particular, may help
inform current trends regarding
aggregate drinking rates.
Although the distributions of
these characteristics have
changed over time, the
associations among the variables
should be relatively stable and
applicable to current behavior.
More recent population rates on
key variables will be offered
where appropriate.
Quantity and frequency drinking
variables revealed that nearly
half of the respondents did not
drink at all in the past year
and less than 4% typically drank
four or more drinks, resulting
in highly skewed distributions
for these drinking
variables. A dichotomous
"Drinking Status" variable was
constructed and coded "1" for
any drinking in the past year or
"0" for no drinking in the past
year. A "Drinking Level"
variable was also constructed,
for drinkers only, which
included aspects of quantity and
frequency of alcohol
consumed. This variable
was a product of the number of
drinking days per month and the
number of drinks typically drunk
per drinking occasion. The
resulting value was divided by
30 to give mean drinks per
day. This was then dummy
coded into "light" drinkers
(< .5 drinks per day) and
"moderate/heavy" (.5 + drinks
per day). Although there
are many measurements and
definitions of light drinking,
this one is consistent with the
CDC's definition of light
drinking. There were
59 cases with missing values on
the drinking variables; these
were dropped from the sample.
Age was measured in years and
gender was coded as male (1) or
female (0). Marital status was
coded as currently married (1)
and not currently married (0),
to capture the social roles of
marriage. Education was coded as
any college (1) or no college
(0). This measure was chosen to
reflect a college culture
experience as well as a broad
proxy for socioeconomic
status. Similarly, the
role or social experience of
paid employment was measured
with "work" coded as currently
employed (1) and not currently
employed (0). Presence of
children in the household was
re-coded from a variable
measuring number of children
under age 18 in the
household. Over half the
respondents had no children
under 18, again resulting in a
non-normal distribution, so this
variable was coded
dichotomously; any children
under 18 currently living in the
home (1) or none (0). The
hours of household labor scale
was constructed by summing the
scores from 9 items asking about
hours spent per week on
preparing meals, washing dishes,
cleaning house, outdoor tasks,
shopping, washing/ironing,
paying bills, auto maintenance,
and driving.
Gender role attitudes were
measured with a scale
constructed from responses to
five items concerning gendered
family responsibilities; "It is
much better for everyone if the
man earns the main living and
the woman takes care of the home
and family," "It is all right
for children under three years
old to be cared for all day in a
day care center," "Preschool
children are likely to suffer if
their mother is employed," "It
is all right for mothers to work
full-time when their youngest
child is under age 5," and "Both
the husband and wife should
contribute to family
income." Responses for
these items ranged from strongly
agree (1) to strongly disagree
(5) and response codes were
selectively reverse coded so
that higher scores reflected
more traditional attitudes. Item
scores were summed and divided
by 5; final scores ranged from 1
to 5.
The objector religion variable
was constructed by re-coding an
item asking for respondents'
religious preference. There were
66 response categories for this
item. Items were coded as
objector religions (1) if
doctrine prohibited (not just
discouraged) alcohol use. All
Evangelical, Muslim, and known
fundamentalist responses were
coded as objector religions as
well as religions for which a
formal statement on alcohol use
could be located. Religions for
which alcohol use beliefs could
not be ascertained or where
alcohol use was merely
discouraged, were coded as
non-objector (0). Metropolitan
status was recoded to represent
metropolitan (1) or
non-metropolitan and adjacent
(0). Region of Country was
re-coded as south (1) or all
others (0).
Ethnicity included three
categories; African American,
white, and any Hispanic.
The African American and
Hispanic categories were each
re-coded into dichotomous
variables for use in the
regression analyses with "white"
used as the reference category.
All "other" ethnic groups and
those with missing values on
ethnicity were dropped from the
analysis (n=133). The
final sample size was 9,872.
Analysis
The main analyses were conducted
using SPSS version 19.
First, percentages (or means)
for all drinking, role,
attitude, and demographic
variables by gender and ethnic
group were calculated to
describe the sample and the
distribution of these
characteristics (Table 1).
Table
1
Descriptives
of Drinking Status, Drinking Level, Role,
Demographic and Attitude by Variables by
Ethnicity and Gender
Men
Women
N
(% in race)
White
Blk
Hisp
Total
White
Blk
Hisp.
Total
Drinker
59.8
47.5
55.1
58.2
43.5
26.7
26.1
40.3
9,807
College
53.9
34.0
30.6
50.1
44.1
35.7
29.6
42.1
9,850
Work
75.6
70.6
76.1
75.1
57.0
57.6
56.0
57.0
9,872
Married
73.0
49.7
62.6
69.9
63.1
32.5
52.5
58.7
9,870
Kids at home
34.7
34.5
52.2
36.1
36.2
49.6
55.6
39.2
9,872
Mean hrs/wk
housework
12.5
18.4
12.4
13.1
30.6
34.4
39.9
31.7
9,220
Mean age
47.6
44.6
42.1
46.8
49.5
45.6
43.5
48.6
9,869
South
31.6
55.4
32.2
34.1
32.4
54.9
31.9
35.0
9,843
Metro
79.2
87.9
97.6
81.5
78.2
88.4
97.6
80.8
9,841
Objector Rel.
24.4
69.0
12.7
27.9
27.2
73.0
11.9
31.3
9,802
Mean Trad.Attitude
3.14
2.84
3.24
3.12
3.02
2.74
3.04
2.99
9,388
>Lightdrinka
29.3
21.2
21.6
27.7
18.3
13.8
15.5
17.6
4,786
Column %
39.0
4.8
3.8
47.6
42.5
6.1
3.9
52.4
(100)
N=
3852
472
372
4696
4195
599
383
5176
9872
a Among
drinkers only
NSFH, Wave
II, 1993, weighted by normalized
Wave II total person weight
(Wave I total weight, tracing
correction, household
correction, post-stratification
correction). Gender
attitude scale; higher values
are more traditional
To examine the
effects of the role, attitude
and demographic variables on
Drinking Status
(drinker/non-drinker) and
Drinking Amount (light/other)
two sets of logistic regression
models were run; one each for
men and women. Logistic
regression models are suited for
analyses with dichotomous
dependent variables and results
are expressed as logged odds
ratios that represent the
increase or decrease in odds of
obtaining the dependent variable
value (1), given the values of
the independent variables.
Total model effects are
expressed as a Chi square
statistic that tests the
increase in model fit from the
addition of the independent
variables. The first
set of models regressed Drinking
Status on ethnicity, college,
employed, metro area, age,
married, kids, household labor
hours, objector religion,
Southern region, and gender-role
attitudes for men and women
separately (Table 2).
Next, Drinking Amount was
regressed on those same role,
attitude, and demographic
variables for men and for women
(Table 3).
Table 2
Logistic
Regression of Drinking Status on Explanatory
Variables, by Gender
MenWomen
B
SE
exp(B)
B
SE
exp(B)
Afric. Amer.
-.069
.063
.933
-.210
.060
.810***
Hispanic
-.239
.133
.787
-1.043
.140
.352***
College
.363
.072
1.438***
.342
.069
1.408***
Employed
.435
.096
1.545***
.177
.076
1.194*
Married
.019
.089
1.019
.103
.072
1.108
Kids at home
-.190
.086
.827*
-.365
.084
.694***
HH Labor hrs
-.007
.003
.993
-.005
.002
.995**
Age
-.017
.003
.983***
-.034
.003
.967***
South
-.194
.075
.824**
-.246
.072
.782**
Metro Area
.175
.090
1.191
.380
.087
1.462***
Religion
-1.007
.082
.365***
-.938
.081
.391***
Trad. Attitude
-.316
.051
.729***
-.282
.048
.754***
(Constant)
2.108
.247
2.287
.247
Model Chi
Square=
548.97***
811.30***
Nagelkerke R2=
.168
.218
N=
3372
5378
*<.05,
**<.01, ***<.001
NSFH, Wave
II, 1993, weighted by normalized
Wave II total person weight
(Wave I total weight, tracing
correction, household
correction, post-stratification
correction).
Drinking
status; drink in past year (1),
no drinking in past year (0)
Table 3
Logistic
Regression of Drinking Level on Explanatory
Variables, by Gender
MenWomen
B
SE
exp(B)
B
SE
exp(B)
Afric. Amer.
-.285
.087
.752***
-.223
.121
.800
Hispanic
-.204
.163
.816
-.056
.249
.946
College
-.271
.087
.762***
.056
.110
1.058
Employed
.037
.130
1.038
-.176
.127
.838
Married
-.452
.106
.636***
-.175
.112
.840
Kids at home
.090
.101
1.094
-.536
.127
.585***
HH Labor hrs
.001
.004
1.001
.004
.003
1.004
Age
.001
.004
1.001
-.004
.004
.996
South
.258
.093
1.294***
.077
.119
1.080
Metro Area
.178
.114
1.195
.336
.157
1.399*
Religion
-.204
.119
.815
-.399
.159
.671*
Trad. Attitudes
-.237
.063
.798***
.012
.075
.988
(Constant)
.823
.307
-.680
.380
Model Chi
Square=
68.67***
52.59***
Nagelkerke R2=
.034
.039
N=
2026
2193
*<.05,
**<.01, ***<.001
NSFH, Wave
II, 1993, weighed by
normalized Wave II total
person weight (Wave I total
weight, tracing correction,
household correction, and
post-stratification
correction).
Drinking
level; light (0), mod/heavy
(1)
For both
descriptive and logistic
regression analyses, the data
were weighted to correct for
tracing, stratification, and
response biases in the original
data collection at Wave I and in
Wave II. These weights
were normalized to retain the
sample size. There is debate
about the utility of weights for
regression analyses using the
NSFH (Johnson & Elliott,
1998; Fox, Benson, Demaris,
& Van Wyk, 2002).
However, the descriptive
analysis in the current study
explores the population
distribution of several
variables and relationships, so
sampling weights were used in
these analyses. These
weights were also retained in
the regression analyses because
variables in the models
(especially ethnicity) were
likely to be affected by the
over-sampling.
Design effects concerning
possible biases from cluster
sampling homogeneity were
explored with a post-hoc
analysis of the logistic
regression models using the NSFH
sampling unit information and
Stata data analysis software.
These design effects do not
alter coefficient estimates but
can increase (or decrease)
standard errors, possibly
overestimating statistical
significance. Again, the utility
of these design effect
corrections are debated; it is
argued whether the estimate
benefits outweigh the
compromises in power. The
regression models used in this
study include the main NSFH
cluster-generating variables
(region, metropolitan area,
ethnicity) so additional
cluster-design corrections were
not deemed
necessary.
Results
The descriptive results (Table
1) generally reflect previous
research regarding drinking
among US gender and ethnic
groups. For both Drinking Status
and Drinking Level, whites have
the highest rates, followed by
Hispanics and then African
Americans. Men drank more
than women in each case but the
gender gap was widest among
Hispanics, followed by African
Americans, and narrowest among
whites. Also reflecting
previous research, Hispanics had
the lowest rates of objector
religious identity (Hispanics
were predominantly catholic),
the highest rates of traditional
work/family roles and the most
traditional gender role
attitudes. Consistent with
previous research, the
descriptive results also show
that older age, female gender,
objector religious
identification, and southern
region were associated with
decreased drinking, and college
and employment were associated
with higher drinking
rates.
The logistic regression analyses
that regressed Drinking Status
on attitude, role, and
demographic variables showed
that for both men and women,
college and employment were
positively associated with
drinking, while age, objector
religion, southern residence,
children at home, housework
hours and traditional
gender-role attitudes were
associated with abstinence for
both men and women (table
2). For men there were no
independent effects of ethnicity
on drinking status after
controlling for other variables
in the model. For women, both
African American and Hispanic
ethnicity were positively
associated with
abstinence. Metro
residence was positively
associated with drinking for
women only. The regression
model for women explained more
variation in Drinking Status
than did the men's model.
The logistic regression analysis
of Drinking Level (Table 3)
included only drinkers and
showed that, for men, African
American ethnicity, college,
marriage, and traditional
gender-role attitudes were
associated with lower drinking
levels. Among these
drinking men, Southern residence
was associated with higher
drinking levels. For women
drinkers, children in the
household and objector religion
were associated with lower
drinking levels and metro
residence was associated with
higher drinking levels.
Discussion
The effects of housework hours
on Drinking Status were
significant and similar for men
and women. However, the
difference in mean housework
hours between men and women was
substantial; 13.1 hrs/wk for men
vs. 31.7 for women (Table
1). These data are from
1993 and current population
trends show that men's housework
contribution and family role
participation has increased
steadily (Cunningham,
2007). If this
relationship between housework
hours and drinking holds,
aggregate decreases men's
drinking could be due, in part,
to increases in their housework
hours. From a role perspective,
it may be that housework hours
themselves limit opportunities
to drink or they may reflect a
broader sense of responsibility
for family roles that is not
compatible with drinking.
Although housework was
associated with Drinking Status,
it was not associated with
Drinking Level for either men or
women. This pattern
suggests that the housework
variable taps a sense of
responsibility rather than a
time constraint.
The parental role was associated
with increased abstinence for
both men and women but the
effects were substantially
stronger for women. This may
reflect the persistently
gendered responsibilities of
family roles in which women are
more likely to be the primary
caretaker (Bianchi, Milkie,
Sayer & Robinson,
2000). Child care is the
family role most closely linked
to a social structural
explanation for gender
differences in drinking.
The time, venue, and social
constraints associated with
full-time child-care can be
functionally, or apparently,
incompatible with drinking. At
any rate there is a heavy stigma
attached to drinking mothers
(Waterson, 2000).
These parental status findings
are limited in that they did not
use a measure of child care
hours or specific
responsibilities but rather used
a measure indicating whether
there were children under 18 in
the household. Although data
concerning hours of child-care
and number of children would
provide more detail, it may also
be the responsibility associated
with the role of primary
caregiver that affects this
relationship between parenthood
and drinking. To further
explore the effects of
parenthood on drinking, future
research should explore the
distinction between own- and
step-children in the household
and should include own children
living in another
household. Children are
more likely to live in their
mother's household and there may
be gender differences in felt
responsibility for step-children
living in one's own home. The
strength and gender discrepancy
in these effects, along with
recent trends regarding men's
family role participation,
warrant further exploration of
the effects of changing
work-family roles on men's
drinking behavior. Among
drinkers, the effect of children
in the home had a substantial
negative effect on amount of
drinking for women only.
Again, this may reflect the
primary caregiver role that
women are more likely to occupy.
Marriage had no effect on
Drinking Status for either men
or women. It may be that other
variables in the model accounted
for factors typically associated
with abstinence among married
individuals, or perhaps couples
self-select into similar
Drinking Status pairs. Although
marriage had no effect on
abstinence it was associated
with substantially lower
drinking levels among drinking
men. As discussed in previous
research, marriage may offer a
protective effect through social
support and active
discouragement of harmful
behaviors. However, these
protective effects may
themselves entail gendered
behaviors if women are expected
to be responsible for their
husbands' drinking
behavior. Or, husbands may
simply assimilate to their wives
drinking patterns.
Employment was positively
associated with Drinking Status
for both men and women but the
association was substantially
stronger for men. Although the
diverse social networks,
drinking norms, personal income
and drinking opportunities
provided by employment may
encourage or enable drinking,
the gender differences here
suggest that this relationship
is weaker for women than for
men. Possible intervening
factors include women's
additional family
responsibilities, occupational
culture, and gender segregation
of the workplace. At any rate,
this pattern does not offer
strong support for a stress
explanation for women's
employment and drinking.
Stress effect drinking is likely
to be heavier drinking and in
this analysis, employment was
associated with lower levels of
drinking among drinking
women. These findings
suggest an increased likelihood
of drinking with women's
employment but also an increased
likelihood of light drinking
rather than moderate or heavy
drinking, among employed
drinkers.
College experience was
associated with an increased
likelihood of drinking for both
men and women. The College
experience provides the
environment, social
opportunities and diverse
network exposure that encourages
drinking and for many, it is a
time of few responsibilities
that would constrain
drinking. However, in this
analysis, the college variable
reflects a past role experience
(for most respondents) that
would most accurately reflect
exposure to the college drinking
culture. Although this
past college experience is
associated with increased
likelihood of current drinking,
among men who do drink, it is
associated with lower levels of
drinking. This may
suggest a protective effect of
past heavy drinking on current
drinking, or it may simply
reflect socioeconomic status.
However, most research shows
that drinking is positively
associated with socioeconomic
status. The implications of this
finding are fairly substantial.
These long-term effects of the
college experience on drinking
should be investigated further.
This quantitative exploration of
ethnicity does not delve into
the large body of literature on
the cultural, political, and
historical aspects of ethnicity,
religion and drinking (Bales,
1946; Christmon, 1995; Gilbert,
1991; Gordon, 1985; Herd, 1985;
Rebach, 1992; Collins &
McNair, 2004). However,
several researchers have
suggested that there is an
over-reliance on cultural
explanations for ethnic
differences in drinking patterns
that can otherwise be explained
by social structural factors
(Darrow, Russell, Cooper, Mudar,
& Frone, 1992; Gilbert &
Collins, 1997; Guttmann, 1999;
Hunt & Barker, 2001;
Nyaronga, Greenfield &
McDaniel, 2009; Nielsen,
2000). In this analysis,
the lack of significant effects
of ethnicity on Drinking Status
among men, and drinking level
among women, suggests that other
variables in that model account
for all of the ethnic
differences in those models.
However, ethnicity remains a
strong predictor of women's
abstinence, and Drinking Level
among African American
men. The effects of
Hispanic ethnicity on women's
drinking were among the
strongest in that model.
These results suggest a complex
interaction between gender and
ethnicity that is not explained
by simple ethnicity effects.
Objector religion, Southern
region, and traditional gender
role attitudes comprise a group
of variables generally
associated with restrictive vs.
liberal attitudes regarding both
drinking and gender roles. All
of these variables were
significantly associated with
increased abstinence for both
men and women. Objector religion
was one of the strongest
predictors of abstinence in the
models. It also appears
that these variables represent
independent effects which vary
substantially by
ethnicity. The descriptive
results show that African
Americans have the highest rates
of southern residence and
objector religious membership,
yet the least traditional gender
role attitudes. Hispanics have
the most traditional gender role
attitudes yet the lowest rates
of objector religious
affiliation. The ethnic
variability and independent
effects of these variables speak
to their importance in
epidemiological research on
drinking. It may be that
some of the effects of ethnicity
on drinking found in the
literature are due to
differences in these demographic
and attitude variables.
The attitude-related variables
did not have as strong of an
effect on Drinking Level as they
did on Drinking Status.
Objector religion had a weak but
significant negative association
with drinking level among women,
and traditional gender role
attitudes reduced men's drinking
substantially. Southern
residence, although strongly
associated with increased
abstinence for both men and
women, was associated with
increased drinking among men who
did drink. A common explanation
for this type of effect is that
the lack of a model of for light
normative drinking in the
formative years will lead to
heavy and problem drinking among
those who do drink. However,
this effect was not observed in
any of the other high-abstinence
groups such as ethnic minorities
or objector religious
affiliates. African
American men who drank were more
likely than white men to be
light drinkers. Hispanic women,
the group least likely to drink,
and African American women, did
not differ from white women in
the amount they drank, if they
did drink.
Finally, many of the factors
found to affect Drinking Status
and Drinking Level in these
analyses may account, at least
in part, for gendered trends in
aggregate drinking rates.
Current US demographic trends
show increased population shares
among the groups with lower
drinking rates in this analysis;
the percent of the US population
in the 45-64 age range increased
31.5% between 2000 and 2010,
whereas the 18-44 age group
increased only 0.6% (US Bureau
of the Census, 2011b), the
Hispanic population grew 43%
between 2000 and 2010, compared
to 10% for the total US
population (US Bureau of the
Census 2011a), the US south is
experiencing disproportionately
rapid population growth (US
Bureau of the Census, 2011c) and
there is relative stability in
objector religious denominations
as opposed to declines in other
denominations (Kosmin &
Keysar, 2009). Although these
factors are rarely addressed in
studies concerned with
individual drinking problem risk
factors, they could have a large
impact on aggregate drinking
rates through population growth
trends in the high-abstinence
groups.
Conclusions
The effects of objector
religious affiliation on
drinking status were substantial
for both men and women and the
rate of participation in
objector denominations was
relatively high (averaging over
25%) and varied widely by
ethnicity. These findings
confirm the need to consider
this factor in drinking research
analyses especially those
looking at ethnic
differences. Gender role
attitudes also offered
additional, independent effects
on drinking status for both men
and women. Although these
variables were clearly
associated with drinking status,
their effects on drinking
quantity among drinkers were
less clear and varied by gender;
traditional gender role
attitudes had a strong negative
effect on drinking quantity
among drinking men.
Employment, any college,
housework and children were all
associated with drinking status
for men and women, although
rates of participation in these
roles varied along traditional
gender role lines. The
research regarding women's
drinking is often framed as an
assimilation affect in which
women are assimilating to men's
drinking patterns as they move
into traditionally male
employment patterns. Findings
here suggest that the influence
of changing roles on drinking
patterns may be
reciprocal. The family
role constraints that have
traditionally limited women's
drinking may limit men's
drinking as they increase their
participation in family roles
(although the distinction
between family role
participation and primary
caretaker may remain an
important distinction).
Increasingly gender-segregated
work and family spheres are
likely to produce new drinking
norms influenced by the gender
composition of these changing
networks.
Drinking rates regarding gender,
age, ethnicity, and region found
in the literature were confirmed
here. Coupled with differential
population growth patterns among
these groups, current decreases
in aggregate drinking rates may
be due to demographic trends
that correspond to group
drinking rates. In order
to avoid an ecological fallacy
these relationships need to be
explored with current
representative longitudinal data
that include adequate drinking
measures. The effects of
ethnicity appeared to be
accounted for by other variables
in two of the models; men's
drinking status and women's
drinking quantity, but there
also appear to be complex
relationships among ethnicity,
gender, objector religion and
gender-role attitudes that
suggest an
intersectionality not modeled
here. One critique of
ethnicity research which this
study was unable to surmount was
the use of broad "ethnic"
categories to define vastly
heterogeneous groups. The
"Hispanic" category in the NSFH
data contains individuals of
diverse national heritage and
cultures. Retaining these
distinctions would leave small
ethnic group sample sizes that
would be difficult to interpret
in this context. A
qualitative exploration might
yield helpful insights regarding
ethnicity, gender, and
drinking.
Finally, one result here was
inconsistent with the literature
on drinking patterns; among
drinkers, men who had attended
college were more likely to be
light drinkers than those who
had not. The reasons for
this are unclear but if this
effect is replicated, it may be
interpreted as a protection
effect of past heavy
drinking. This effect does
need further exploration.
References
Ames, G., & Rebhun, L.
(1996). "Women, Alcohol and Work:
Interactions of Gender, Ethnicity,
and Occupational Culture." Social
Science and Medicine 43(11),
1649-1663.
Bales, R.F. (1946). "Cultural
Differences in Rates of
Alcoholism." Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol
6:480-499.
Bergmark, K.H. (2004). "Gender
Roles, Family, and Drinking: Women
at the Crossroad of Drinking
Cultures." Journal of
Family History 29 (3),
293-307.
Bianchi, S.M., Milkie, M., Sayer
L.C., & Robinson J.P.
(2000). "Is Anyone Doing the
Housework?; Trends in the Gender
Division of Household Labor." Social
Forces 79(1), 191-228.
Caetano, R., & Clark C.L.
(1999). "Trends in Alcohol
Consumption Patterns among Whites,
Blacks and Hispanics: 1984 and
1995." Drug and Alcohol
Dependence 54(1), 45-56.
Caetano, R. & Clark C.L.
(1998). "Trends in Alcohol
Consumption Patterns among Whites,
Blacks, and Hispanics:
1984-1995." Journal on
Studies of Alcohol 659-668.
Christmon, K. (1995).
"Historical Overview of Alcohol in
the African American Community."
Journal of Black Studies
25(3), 318-330.
Collins, R.L., & McNair, L.D.
(2004). "Minority Women and
Alcohol Use." Retrieved from
the Legacy Group on August
30, 2004.
Coltrane, S. (2000).
"Research on Household Labor:
Modeling and Measuring the Social
Embeddedness of Routine Household
Tasks." Journal of
Marriage and the Family
62(4), 1208-1233.
Cunningham, M., (2007).
"Influences of Women's Employment
on the Gendered Division of
Household Labor over the Life
Course: Evidence from a 31-Year
Panel Study." Journal of
Family Issues 28(3),
422-444.
Dawson, D.A., Grant, B.F., Chou,
S.P., & Pickering, R.P.,
(1995). "Subgroup Variation in US
Drinking Patterns: Results of the
1992 National Longitudinal Alcohol
Epidemiologic Study."
Journal of Substance Abuse 7,
331-344.
Darrow, S.L., Russel, M., Cooper,
M.L., Mudar, P. & Frone, M.R.
(1992). "Sociodemographic
Correlates of Alcohol Consumption
among African-American and White
Women." Women and Health
18(4), 35-51.
Fillmore, K.M., Golding, J. M.,
Leino, E.V., Motoyoshi, M.,
Shoemaker, C., Terry, H.,
Ager, C.R. & Ferrer, H.P.
(1997). "Patterns and Trends
in Women's and Men's
Drinking." In Wilsnack, R.W.
& Wilsnack S.C. (Eds.) Gender
and Alcohol: Individual and
Social Perspectives. (pp.
21-47) New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
Center of Alcohol Studies.
Fox, G. L., Benson, M.L.,
Demaris, A.A., & Van Wyk, J.,
(2002). "Economic Distress
and Intimate Violence: Testing
Family Stress and Resource
Theories." Journal of
Marriage and Family 64,
793-807.
Gilbert, M. J. (1985).
"Mexican Americans in California:
Intracultural Variation in
Attitudes and Behavior Related to
Alcohol." In Bennett, L.A. and
Ames, M.A. (Eds.) The American
Experience with Alcohol:
Contrasting Cultural
Perspectives. (pp. 255-277)
New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Gilbert, M.J. (1991).
"Acculturation and Changes in
Drinking Patterns among
Mexican-American Women." Alcohol
Health & Research World
15(3), 234-241.
Gilbert, M.J. & Collins R.L.
(1997). "Ethnic Variation in
Women's and Men's Drinking."
In Wilsnack, R.W. and Wilsnack
S.C. (Eds.) Gender and
Alcohol: Individual and Social
Perspectives. (pp. 357-377)
New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Center
of Alcohol Studies.
Gordon, A.J. (1985). "Alcohol and
Hispanics in the Northeast A Study
of Cultural Variability and
Adaptation." In Bennett,
L.A. and Ames, M.A. (Eds.) The
American Experience with
Alcohol: Contrasting Cultural
Perspectives. (pp.
297-313) New York, NY: Plenum
Press.
Gutmann, M.C. (1999).
"Ethnicity, Alcohol, and
Acculturation." Social
Science and Medicine 48,
173-184.
Ham, L.S., & Hope, D.A.
(2003). "College Students and
Problematic Drinking: A Review of
the Literature." Clinical
Psychology Review 23,
719-759.
Hammer, T. & Vaglum, P.
(1989) "The Increase in
Alcohol Consumption among Women: a
Phenomenon Related to
Accessibility or Stress?; A
General Population Study." British
Journal of Addiction 84:
67-775
Herd, D. (1985). "Ambiguity
in Black Drinking Norms: An
Ethnohistorical Interpretation."
In Bennett, L.A. and Ames, M.A.
(Eds.) The American Experience
with Alcohol: Contrasting
Cultural Perspectives.
(pp. 149-170) New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
Hunt, G. & Barker, J.C.
(2001). "Socio-cultural
Anthropology and Alcohol Research:
Towards a Unified Theory." Social
Science & Medicine
53, 165-188.
Huselid, R.F. & Cooper,
M.L. (1991). "Gender
Roles as Mediators of Sex
Differences in Adolescent Alcohol
Use and Abuse." Paper presented at
the Annual Meeting of the American
Psychological Association, San
Francisco, CA.
Johnson, D. & Elliot, L.
(1998). "Sampling Design Effects:
Do They Affect the Analyses of
Data from the National Survey of
Families and Households?" Journal
of Marriage and Family 60,
993-1001.
Kosmin, B.A. & Keysar, A.
(2009). "American Religious
Identification Survey (ARIS)
2008."
http://b27.cc.trincoll.edu/weblogs/AmericanReligionSurvey-ARIS/
reports/ARIS_Report_2008.pdf.
Hartford, Connecticut, USA:
Trinity College.
Retrieved 5 August 2012.
Leonard, K.E. & Rothbard, J.C.
(1999). "Alcohol and the
Marriage Effect." Journal
of Studies on Alcohol 60,
139-146.
Mirowsky, J. and Ross, C.E.
(1989). Social Causes of
Psychological Distress. New
York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
McPherson, M., Casswell, S. &
Pledger, M. (2004). "Gender
Convergence in Alcohol Consumption
and Related Problems: Issues and
Outcomes from Comparisons of New
Zealand Survey Data."
Addiction 99,
738-748.
Morgan, P. (1987). "Women
and Alcohol: The Disinhibition
Rhetoric in an Analysis of
Domination." Journal of
Psychoactive Drugs 19(2),
129-133.
National Institute on Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism (1994).
"Alcohol and Minorities." Alcohol
Alert 23, p. 347.
National Institute on Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism (2009).
"Percent Distribution of Current
Drinking status, Drinking Levels,
and Heavy Drinking Days by Sex for
Persons 18 Years of Age and Older:
United States." HIS,
1997-2008.
Nielsen, A.L. (2000).
"Examining Drinking Patterns and
Problems among Hispanic Groups:
Results from a National
Survey." Journal of
Studies on Alcohol and Drugs
61, 301-310.
Nyaronga, D., Greenfield, T.K.
& McDaniel, P.A. (2009).
"Drinking Context and Drinking
Problems Among Black, White, and
Hispanic Men and Women in the
1984, 1995, and 2005 U.S. National
Alcohol Surveys." Journal
of Studies on Alcohol &
Drugs 70, 16-26.
Peralta, R.L. (2007).
College Alcohol Use and the
Embodiment of Hegemonic
"Masculinity Among European
American Men." Sex
Roles 56, 741-756.
Peralta, R.L, Steele, J.L.,
Nofziger, S, & Rickles, M.
(2010). "The Impact of
Gender on Binge Drinking Behavior
among U.S. College Students
Attending a Midwestern University:
An Analysis of Two Gender
Measures." Feminist
Criminology 5(4),
355-379.
Rebach, H. (1992). "Alcohol
and Drug use among Minority
African American
Minorities." Ethnic and
Multicultural Drug Abuse 23-57.
Robbins, C. & Martin, S.S.
(1993). "Gender, Styles of
Deviance, and Drinking
Problems." Journal of
Health and Social Behavior 34,
302-321.
Room, R. (1996). "Gender
Roles and Interactions in Drinking
and Drug Use." Journal
of Substance Abuse 2,
227-239.
Sweet, J.A. and Bumpass, L.L.
(1996). "The National Survey
of Families and Households - Waves
1 and 2: Data Description and
Documentation." Center for
Demography and Ecology, University
of Wisconsin-Madison
(http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/nsfh/home.htm)
Trotter, R.T. (1985).
"Mexican-American Experience with
Alcohol South Texas
Examples." In Bennett, L.A.
and Ames, M.A. (Eds.) The
American Experience with
Alcohol: Contrasting Cultural
Perspectives. (pp.
279-296) New York, NY: Plenum
Press.
U.S. Bureau of the Census.
(2011a). "The Hispanic
Population: 2010, 2010."
Census Briefs,
http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-04.pdf
U.S. Bureau of the Census.
(2011b). "Age and Sex Composition:
2010, 2010." Census Briefs,
http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-03.pdf.
U.S. Bureau of the Census.
(2011c). "Population Distribution
and Change: 2000 to 2010, 2010."
Census Briefs,
http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-01.pdf
Wallace, J.M., Bachman,
J.G., O'Malley, P.M., Schulenberg,
J.E., Cooper, S.M. & Johnston,
L.D. (2003). "Gender and
Ethnic Differences in Smoking,
Drinking, and Illicit Drug use
among African Americans 8th, 10th,
and 12th Grade Students,
1976-2000." Addiction
98, 225-234.
Waterson, J. (2000). Women
and Alcohol in Social Context.
Hampshire, UK: Palgrave
Publishers, St Martin's Press.
Wilsnack, R.W. & Cheloha, R.
(1987) "Women's Roles and Problem
Drinking Across the
Lifespan." Social
Problems 34(3),
231-248.
Wilsnack, R.W. & Wilsnack,
S.C. (1992). "Women, Work, and
Alcohol: Failures of Simple
Theories." Alcoholism:
Clinical and Experimental
Research 16(2), 172-179.
Wilsnack, R.W., Kristjanson, A.F.,
Wilsnack, S.C. & Crosby, R.D.
(2006). "Are U.S. Women Drinking
Less (or More)? Historical and
Aging Trends, 1981-2001." Journal
of Studies on Alcohol 67,
341-348.
Wilsnack, R.W., Wilsnack, S.C.,
Kristjanson, A.F. ,
Vogeltanz-Holm, N.D. & Gmel,
G. (2009). "Gender and Alcohol
Consumption: Patterns from the
Multinational GENACIS
Project." Addiction104, 1487-1500.
Sociation Today is optimized for the
Firefox Browser
The Editorial Board of Sociation Today
Editorial Board:
Editor:
George H. Conklin,
North Carolina
Central University
Emeritus
Robert Wortham,
Associate Editor,
North Carolina
Central University
Board:
Rebecca Adams,
UNC-Greensboro
Bob Davis,
North Carolina
Agricultural and
Technical State
University
Catherine Harris,
Wake Forest
University
Ella Keller,
Fayetteville
State University
Ken Land,
Duke University
Steve McNamee,
UNC-Wilmington
Miles Simpson,
North Carolina
Central University
William Smith,
N.C. State University