Sociation
Today®
ISSN 1542-6300
The Official Journal of the
North Carolina Sociological
Association
A Peer-Reviewed
Refereed Web-Based
Publication
Spring/Summer 2015
Volume 13, Issue 1
Perceptions
of Music Majors and Music
Performance Students on
the Use of Music as
Torture in the War on
Terror
by
John Paul
and
Stephanie K. Decker
Washburn University
Introduction
The American
military's use of music and
sound as a technique of
interrogation and as a vehicle
of forced compliance on
prisoners in the "global war on
terror" has been well documented
by journalists and by select
scholars of music (Cusick 2006
2008, Pieslak 2007).
Specifically, it has been
reported that music has been
used to break prisoners'
resistance to interrogation
"through sleep deprivation,
disorientation, and playing
music that was culturally
offensive to them" (BBC News
2003:1).
Given
these practices, and following
Cusick's (2006) initial query
wherein she asked the
"blogosphere" (comprised of
musicians and/or music
educators) to reflect upon the
idea of music as torture, we ask
a very specific segment of the
public (music students at a
Midwestern American University)
if they are aware of these
practices and what they make of
music being used in these ways.
Pragmatically, we chose to query
music majors and performers
because we felt this topic would
resonate with them in a personal
way, as they are deeply
intertwined in the realm of
music consumption, study, and
production. Further, as Bohlman
(2007) notes, the student
musician and the student
ethnomusicologist should have a
crucial presence in these
debates about the role of music
as torture. He states:
For [students
of music], ethics does not
inhabit only everyday
practice. It shapes our
identities, how we and others
see us as citizens in
communities that overlap and
intersect, our own musical
worlds... and the public
sphere from which [students of
music] should not distance
[themselves] (2007:1).
Additionally, from the Papeti and
Grant (2013:1-2):
The
presumption that music is an
invariably an uplifting and
ennobling art form is well
established and dates back to
antiquity…[yet] despite the
tendency to focus on music's
benign and positive role, we
are confronted today with
clear disclosures of its role
in torture and human rights
violations. Recent revelations
of music's use in the
detention and interrogation
centers of the so-called 'War
on Terror' have underlined
music's potential to wound and
cause suffering... [Thus
a] key question [must be
asked]: Can music be
considered a form of torture?
[This] difficult and
contentious question [has] not
gone unnoticed by [scholars],
although it must be said that
musicology as a discipline has
mostly passed over them in
silence...More documentation
and debate on this subject is
crucial...
Heeding
the call from these scholars, we
offer this study as a small
contribution to this emergent
area of "music as torture." As
noted, the student voice has
been largely overlooked in these
debates about the unconventional
and controversial uses of music.
By querying the student
musician, we hope to gain
insight into their depth of
knowledge regarding this
practice, and per Bohlman's
(2007) and Papeti and Grant's
(2013) request, we offer a
collection of underrepresented
voices to this
debate.
Given
that there is little previous
sociological or
ethno-musicological research on
music students' reactions to
music as torture, this research
may also be described as an
exploratory study. Thus, in part
one of this paper we offer a
history of music as weaponry and
we detail the reality of music
as torture. In part two, we turn
to the exploratory portion and
we offer a description of our
methodology as well as the five
core themes that emerged from
our study with music students,
namely: limited familiarity,
questioning the idea of music as
torture, assuming torture was
only used on "bad guys", belief
in the efficacy of torture, and
altered relationships with
music. Lastly a discussion
including the limitations,
implications, and directions for
future research is provided.
Examples
of Music as a Weapon
While journalists and human
rights organizations have
documented the use of music in
torture, few scholars (with
exceptions: Cusick 2006 2008,
Pieslak 2007, Papeti and Grant
2013) have examined the issue.
The use of music as both
weaponry and an instrument of
torture by American authorities
have been documented by
journalists even before the
"global war on terror" (for a
detailed discussion of music as
weaponry in non American
settings, see Cloonan and
Johnson 2002). Prior to the "war
on terror" the most well known
examples were the musical
bombardment of Panamanian
dictator Manuel Noriega in
1990 to hasten his
surrender to American soldiers
(Potter 1998) and the "sound
siege" of the Branch Davidian
Compound in Waco Texas in 1993
to disorient cult members prior
to its raid by federal
agents (Ammerman, 1993).
Speaking first to the Noriega
conflict, we reference the words
of Potter (1998:37-8), who
writes:
Music was used
to inflict pain [as well as]
annoy the papal staff of the
Vatican embassy in Panama City
were Noriega was seeking
sanctuary. The goal was to
apply pressure to the Vatican
staff hoping it would compel
the Monsignor to release
Noriega… The music was
selected to be repetitive and
loud, although it included a
number of topical pieces such
as 'No Place to Run' and
'You're no Good.' In this case
there was also the aesthetic
dimension to the attack, as
Noriega was an Opera
lover.
In the
end, however, this tactic
backfired as U.S. Catholics and
Vatican officials saw the
practice as a "clumsy effort to
harass Noriega and inflict
needless stress upon the papal
staff" (Rouse, 2000:1). As a
result President George Herbert
Walker Bush instructed military
officials to silence the music.
Moving
next to the Davidian siege, we
identify Albert and Bell's
(2002) rather strange but
intriguing article that examines
the Waco tragedy within the
language of music theory. We
paraphrase and "translate" the
authors' arguments below:
After
bombarding the Davidians with
deafening and disorienting
sounds, only silence was
returned…the negotiators grew
uncomfortable with the level
of silence deciding that "the
orchestra piece" they and
Davidians were conducting was
now in the "final movement."
Federal agents determined "to
hit the last note" preceded
their raid on the compound
with tear gas, unintentionally
sparking fires [or encouraging
arson by the Davidians] that
quickly engulfed the structure
and the majority of the Branch
Davidians within.
Though
the aforementioned article stops
short of a critique of the
Federal government's role in the
Waco tragedy, it nonetheless
implies a negative assessment of
the use of music to penetrate
the Davidians' compound. No
longer was music simply a medium
for entertainment or apolitical
aesthetic pleasure. Instead, as
Albert and Bell (2002) imply, it
became a tool of disorientation
and subjugation, a hymn to
disguise cunning, and a marching
tune of overconfidence.
Music
has also played a role in the
torture of detainees in the
"global war on terror." the use
of music as a technique of
interrogation was reported on as
early as 2003 (with the US
invasion and occupation of
Iraq). Three months following
the American invasion and
occupation of Iraq, the BBC
(2003) issued a report detailing
the US Army's use of music (e.g.
Metallica's "Enter Sandman" and
Barney the Purple Dinosaur's "I
Love You") in the interrogation
of terror suspects. The
songs were repeatedly played at
high volume inside confined
spaces with the goal of breaking
prisoners' resistance to
interrogation "through sleep
deprivation, disorientation, and
playing music that was
culturally offensive to them"
(BBC News 2003:1).
This
tactic was confirmed by Piore
(2003), who notes that US
interrogators used heavy metal
and American's children's songs
to "break the will" of
uncooperative captives. Citing a
US Psychological Operations
specialist, he writes:
These people
haven't heard heavy metal.
They can't take it. If you
play it for 24 hours, your
brain and body function start
to slide, your train of
thought starts to slow down
and your will is broken.
That's when we come in and
talk to them (Piore, 2003:13).
Pieslak
(2007:132) offers additional
evidence of the use of music to
inflict pain on detainees when,
in transcribing the recorded
words of a military
interrogator, he writes "that
songs were used to get on these
people's nerves so that…their
resistance was weakened…[in one
such case] a tape of babies
crying worked so well that
detainees usually answered
questions after half an hour of
listening."
The use
of music as a vehicle of
compliance still exists despite
the stated policy shift from the
Obama Administration. Shortly
after the inauguration of
President Obama, the White House
announced that the use of loud
music was one of the techniques
tossed away during a policy
change on interrogations. Yet,
as Cusick and Joseph (2011: 8-9)
write, "the use of sound is now
described as a condition of
detention, not a tactic of
interrogation."
When it's
described as a condition of
detention, it's removed from
the usual legal definitions of
torture, which are about
getting information out of
someone… Yet detainees were
not asked questions while
subjected to music… The use of
music to manipulate prisoners'
behavior has always been a
"condition of detention," but
subsuming acoustical violence
at these levels of intensity
under that rubric is [a]
sleight of hand (Cusick and
Joseph (2011: 8-9).
Defining Music
As Torture
While the above cases
demonstrate the documented use
of music as pain, scholarship on
its direct application as
torture is sparse. A key notable
exception is the work by Cusick
and her colleagues (Cusick 2008,
Cusick and Joseph 2011). This
research shows that music may be
used as a medium of pain through
forced listening, sensory
overload, emotional collapse,
and cultural offense. The
continuous, involuntary
listening to music at high
volumes has the effect of
torture simply because it is
forced and inescapable (Cusick,
2008). In addition, she notes
that for detainees the
non-voluntary listening of music
acts as a prison of experience
that "forces a conscious state
of sin that [individuals] are
powerless to avoid" (Cusick,
2008:13-14). In this way, music
can be used to "break" a
prisoner's resistance to
interrogation through sleep
deprivation, disorientation, and
psycho-cultural distress.
Further, though torture is often
conceived as body on body
contact that produces noticeable
external damage, several
scholars challenge the idea of
music being "non-physical." For
instance, as Branden Joseph
writes, "torture [can be
conceptualized as] "body on
body" or "body on body mediated
by something like the acoustical
waves of music" (Cusick and
Joseph 2001: 19). In this way,
the effects of forced listening
may produce anxiety, fatigue,
lack of sleep, disturbances of
mood, attitudes and behavior,
and create ear and nerve damage
(Physicians for Human Rights
2010; Isakson and Jurkovic
2013). Additionally, Cusick
states:
[We] know that
the simple bombardment of a
human body with acoustical
energy will change the body.
It may feel like a beating,
which is what one former
detainee told me several
times: when it's over, he
stated, you feel as though
you've been "beaten with a
hammer." Yet, even if it
doesn't feel like a beating,
every bone in the body of the
person being bombarded with
sound has no choice but to
vibrate sympathetically with
the sound. The entire body is
forced to make music (Cusick
and Joseph 2011: 13).
Furthermore, Cusick (2008) notes
the effects of using music as
social, cultural, and religious
indignation:
When
interrogators confronted [the
detainee] with a Qur'anic
passage declaring [listening
to music] a sin… he 'broke
down crying and asking God for
forgiveness and…stated that he
could do nothing about the
music that was played in his
interrogation booth." (Cusick,
2008:13-14. Words in italics
are our
emphasis).
The
scholarship arguing that the use
of music in interrogation should
be considered torture is
supplemented by work from human
rights organizations and
accounts made by
detainees. Consider the
case of Binyam Mohamed, a
detainee held in Guantánamo Bay,
who reports the following:
The CIA
rendered him to Morocco, where
his torturers repeatedly took
a razor blade to his penis
throughout an 18-month ordeal
[but] psych-ops methods were
worse than this. He could
anticipate physical pain and
know that it would eventually
end. But the experience of
slipping into madness as a
result of torture by music was
something quite different.
Imagine you are given a
choice…Lose your sight or lose
your mind. While having your
eyes gouged out would be
horrendous, there is little
doubt which option you would
choose and what you would be
willing to say for it to end
(Smith 2008:1).
Further, Binyam states:
There were
loudspeakers in the cell,
pumping out what felt like
about 160 watts, a deafening
volume, non-stop, 24 hours a
day. They played the same CD
for a month, "The Eminem
Show." It's got about 20 songs
on it, and when it was
finished, it went back to the
beginning and started again…I
lost my head. It felt like it
was never going to end and
that I had ceased to exist. It
was a miracle my brain is
still intact (Mackey 2009, no
page number).
Former Guantánamo prisoners
Ruhal Ahmed and Shafiq Rasul
recount similar experiences.
First the words of Ruhal:
The physical
actions against me was less
significant than the
relentless, inescapable
noise…I can bear being beaten
up, it's not a problem. Once
you accept that you're going
to go into the interrogation
room and be beaten up, it's
fine. You can prepare yourself
mentally. But when you're
being psychologically
tortured, you can't… [The
music] makes you feel like you
are going mad. You lose the
plot and it's very scary to
think that you might go crazy
because of all the music,
because of the loud noise, and
because after a while you
don't hear the lyrics at all,
all you hear is heavy banging
(Worthington, 2008: no page
number).
Lastly, the words of Shafiq
Rasul:
It just starts
playing with you… Even if you
were shouting, the music was
too loud -- nobody would be
able to hear you. You're there
for hours and hours, and
they're constantly playing the
same music. All that builds
up. You start hallucinating
(Peisner, 2006, no page
number).
U.S.
agents initially accused Rasul
of sitting in on an August 2000
meeting with Osama bin Laden and
lead 9/11 hijacker Mohammed
Atta. Though he initially denied
the charge, Rasul eventually
confessed – due exclusively, he
said, to the torture he endured.
However, investigators would
soon confirm that in August
2000, Shafiq Rasul wasn't with
bin Laden because he was
attending university and working
at an electronics store in
England. In early 2004, Rasul
Ahmed was released without
charges (Peisner, 2006).
Methodology
and Questions
Given these documented causes,
we wanted to know if music
majors and music performance
students were aware of such
practices and whether or not
they perceive music, when used
in this way, as torture. To
obtain this information we
conducted conversational forums
and focus group interviews (see
Krueger and Casey 2009) with
music majors and music
performance students (n=264)
enrolled in six separate
ethnomusicology courses at a
mid-western university across a
six-year time span (2008-2013).
These ethnomusicology courses
were part of the required
curriculum for all music
students at this particular
university. Here, it should be
noted that the first author
participated in these forums as
a scholar in the sociology of
music, while the second author
participated in these forums as
a scholar of war crimes and a
sociology of deviance expert.
Collectively, our scholarly
expertise was employed to help
direct and answer student
questions as they emerged in
these conservations.
In
terms of direct procedure, we
were allowed to conduct
conservational forums with
students during one allotted
class period (a one hour and 15
minute period). We opened the
class by exposing students to
descriptive and documented
vignettes on the US military's
use of music on Iraqi and
Afghani prisoners (see appendix
for vignettes and guiding
questions). After this, students
were asked to reflect aloud on
the vignettes and conceptualize
their views on the idea of
"music as a method of
interrogation." On average, the
forums lasted approximately an
hour with the authors soliciting
and analyzing students' views in
these open discussions. In the
conduction of forums,
conversations would drift
between contrasting views, not
only on the conceptualization of
"what music is for", but also on
the facilitating power of music
to "enable interrogation" and/or
"perform torture." In responding
to student commentary the
authors would strive not to bias
a particular point of view, but
instead probe with questions
like: "What do you think?" "What
do you mean by that?" "Could you
explain this a bit further?"
"Why do you feel/think this
way?" Lastly, conversations
would normally turn to include
discussions of the role and
purpose of music in society, as
well as the personal place of
music in one's own life and
elements of these discussions
were included in the overall
analysis.
Consistent with a qualitative
theme, data collection,
analysis, and theory
construction were done
simultaneously (Glaser and
Strauss, 1967, Strauss and
Corbin, 1990). In the end, we
grouped and analyzed music
students' perspectives within
broad themes guided by
analytical signposts such as:
collective/common agreement,
outliers, turning points in
conservations, recurring
arguments, and intense and
emotional reactions (see also
Strauss and Corbin 1997).
Theme One:
Limited Familiarity
What is overwhelmingly clear
from the focus groups is that
the use of music as an
interrogation technique is
broadly unknown to students. The
majority had not conceived of
music as being used in this way
and most were initially confused
by the concept. The following
statements are representative
across all focus groups and
demonstrate this lack of
familiarity:
What? Are you
serious? That sounds stupid
(Student focus group member
Spring 2008)
Is this made
up? Why would music be used?
What is the point? (Student
focus group member Spring
2009)
Why and how?
(Student focus group member
Spring 2010)
The why—or at least the theory
behind it—as we explained, was
to "break" a prisoner's
resistance to interrogation
through sleep deprivation,
disorientation, and sensory
overload. As, Alfred McCoy
(2006a) a scholar who has
written extensively about the
interrogation methods, explains:
The idea [of
sensory overload] … [with
sound and light] …is that
subjects become so starved for
a break in this stimulation
overload that they will crave
interaction with their
interrogator. The idea is that
they break down and then they
cling to the interrogator
(cited in Benjamin, 2007 no
page number).
Indeed, most persons (including
the authors) were unaware of the
use of music as a technique of
interrogation until various
academic and news reports became
public. Though the use of music
as a technique of interrogation
was reported on as early as 2003
(with the US invasion and
occupation of Iraq), the 2006
Human Rights Watch report was
one of the first to critically
examine its use and make the
case for music existing as a
mode of torture. Their report,
"No blood, No foul" made the
public aware of how music was
being used to psychologically
break detainees as part of the
global "War on Terror." Please
consider the following
description by a former prison
guard at the Forward Operating
Base Tiger near al-Qaim, Iraq:
[A] typical
first time interrogation
consisted of some kind of
heavy metal music really loud,
strobe light, lot of yelled
questions and stuff like that,
until they finally would break
down and cry and say "I don't
know anything, I don't know
anything!" ... He's on his
knees, usually a rifle pointed
at him, strobe light going,
music going, whatever. Then
the guys sitting at the desk
asking him questions directly.
[Everybody always had to yell]
in order to hear [over the
music] (Human Rights Watch,
2006, Soldiers' Accounts -no
page number).
As the
student focus group discussions
continued, and after vignettes
such as the one above were
offered, several students were
able to express familiarity with
the concept of "music as
interrogation" and "music as
torture" via their viewership of
several popular American
television shows.
I saw
something like this on the
show Homeland. They [the CIA]
had a guy [terrorist suspect]
in a room blaring heavy metal
whenever he looked like he was
about to fall asleep (student
focus group member, Spring
2012) … Yeah, this was also on
the TV show Leverage. [In the
episode] former soldiers were
being tortured with music… you
know, sleep deprivation and
using it to drive them crazy
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2012).
Form
here, the conservation typically
shifted to the portrayal of
torture in television shows and
it was noted by many students
that the media formed their
opinions regarding the necessity
for brutal interrogation
tactics. Indeed, in recent
years, the representation of
interrogations in popular media
has increased. The advocacy
group, Human Rights First has
noted that the number of
military-like interrogations
shown on television has grown
from four times in a year before
2001 to more than 100 per year
in the post-9/11 period (Human
Rights First, no date). However,
despite its growth of appearance
in popular culture, and given
the six year span that this
research has been conducted,
students seem no more cognizant
that music has (and is) being
used in this way. Further, the
TV spectacle that depicts "music
as interrogation" is often the
sanitized presentation of real
methods of abuse. Lastly these
televised depictions never raise
moral objections, nor engage in
a serious dialogue on the
subject. As one student said
sarcastically, "…it's not
real, right? It's just a TV
show." (Student group focus
member, Spring 2013).
Theme Two: Questioning the Idea
of Music as Torture
Initially, the majority of our
student musicians doubted the
idea that forced listening could
be harmful. In fact many found
the idea humorous and dismissed
the idea of music as nothing
more than an irritating
encounter:
Torture? I
live in a place that only
plays country music on the
radio – now that's torture!
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2010)
My friend
refuses to let me control [our
listening choices] in the car…
its nothing but hip-hop. I
threatened to jump out of the
car… (Student focus group
member, Spring 2011)…
Emo music…
that stuff is torture. Makes
you want to kill yourself… we
should be making the
terrorists listen to that
stuff. (Student focus group
member, Spring 2008).
The
dismissive attitude toward music
as pain is explained in part
because many people routinely
and humorously refer to the idea
that being forced to listen to a
certain song or music style
apart from their own likes and
tastes is "torture" – therefore,
reports of the tactic of "music
as interrogation" are often cast
in a comic light. Even several
prominent musicians whose music
was used to "soften" detainees
were skeptical. Consider that
James Hetfield, co-founder of
Metallica initially laughed off
the idea of his music being
torture. He joked, "We've been
punishing our parents, our
wives, our loved ones with this
music for ever. Why should the
Iraqis be any different?"
(Smith, 2008:1). Similarly Steve
Asheim, drummer for the
death-metal band Deicide, (who's
song Fuck Your God has been used
by interrogators) questions
whether music really counts as
torture. He states, "If I was a
prisoner at Guantánamo Bay and
they blasted a load of music at
me, I'd be like, 'Is this all
you got? Come on.' I certainly
don't believe in torturing
people, but I don't believe that
playing loud music is torture
either" (Smith, 2008:1). In
addition, a Christian metal band
known as Demon Hunter apparently
got in touch with the US
Military and is reported to have
offered their music as service,
implying that the use of music
in interrogations is not only
acceptable, but honorable. They
stated, "We're all about
promoting what you do…We are
honored, humbled and blessed
that Demon Hunter was of any
support or comfort to Seal Team
6 or anyone in the US military
at any time" (Hogan 2013, no
page number).
Beyond
these collective dismissals,
many also rejected the idea of
music as harmful because they
didn't initially perceive it as
being "physical":
It's just
sound. It can't permanently
hurt you… its not like you are
being beaten up. If I were
being "tortured," give me this
any day (Student focus group
member, Spring 2008).
Ted Nugent
used to say, "if it's too loud
you're too old"… I like loud
music…I'd think of it like a
free concert (Student focus
group member, Spring 2009).
It's not like
they're shooting you in the
kneecaps or hitting you in the
head with a phonebook… its
music played over and over… I
can see where this would be
annoying, but at least you are
coming out [physically intact]
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2010).
Yet, as
detailed earlier, the effects of
forced listening may produce
anxiety, fatigue, lack of sleep,
disturbances of mood, attitudes
and behavior, and create ear and
nerve damage. According to the
Occupational Safety and Health
Administration there is no risk
of permanent hearing loss from
continuous 24-hour-per-day
exposure to noise of up to 82
decibels. The CIA maintains that
exposure to white noise/loud
sounds is not to exceed 79
decibels during interrogations
(Peisner, 2006).
And to
reiterate the words of Cuscik
and Joseph (2011:13),
"acoustical energy [applied to
the body] feels like a beating,
which is what one former
detainee told me several times:
when it's over, he stated, you
feel as though you've been
"beaten with a hammer." Once
students were made aware of
these perspectives and were
offered additional vignettes by
Binyam Mohamed, Ruhal Ahmed and
Shafiq Rasul (as noted earlier
in this paper), several students
altered their initial
reflections:
A person who's
confined and forced to listen
to [noise] against their will
–I believe that would cause
unnatural distress on the mind
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2012).
Even after the
music stops, it can stick with
you. It can also make you feel
a certain way (depressed,
anxious) and it can be an
emotion you can't escape
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2013).
I choose to
listen to music that brings me
pleasure. But if I were made
to listen to something that
offended me, I would be forced
to participate in a music
culture that I did not
choose…that would violate my
spirit (Student focus group
member, Spring 2012).
Theme Three:
Assuming its Use Only on "Bad
Guys"
The majority of our
student-interviewees did not
conceptualize music primarily as
a physical source of pain, and
instead were most apt to be
concerned with its potential as
a source of mental and/or
spiritual harm. Indeed, as our
classroom conservations
continued, many did conclude (or
come to concede to fellow
students) that they felt music,
when used this way, was
undeniably disruptive and
possibly damaging to the mind
and spirit. However, there was
great disagreement as to whether
it was morally right or wrong
and whether it was a valid and
purposive technique for gaining
actionable intelligence from
persons detained in the war on
terror. Data reveal that
students in each of the focus
groups make broad assumptions on
this point. Namely, students
largely assume that persons
being held for interrogation are
"bad people" who would not be in
detention centers if they
weren't guilty of something.
Take for example the following
student remarks:
I admit that
torture is evil, but so are
these people… (Student focus
group member, Spring 2008).
It does not
bother me. It's only torture
if its used on innocent
people… but these are not
innocent people. It doesn't
bother me if they are using
[music] to get information on
people that they suspect has
information (Student focus
group member, Spring 2012).
We speculate that these
attitudes are due in part to the
fact that this was the core
narrative issued by the White
House during the declaration of
the global war on terror. For
instance, the Former Secretary
of Defense Donald Rumsfeld once
declared that individuals
captured by the US military in
the aftermath of 9/11
represented the "worst of the
worst… all were terrorists,
trainers, bomb makers, and
would-be suicide bombers…" (US
Department of Defense, 2005 no
page number).
We shared this
with our student-interviewees
and further reported that the
vast majority of prisoners
captured in the War on Terror
were innocent and the
administration delayed their
freedom because of the political
repercussions that would have
ensued. Consider the words of
Lawrence Wilkerson, former chief
of staff to then Secretary of
State Colin Powell:
I found that …
the majority of detainees had
never seen a US soldier in the
process of their initial
detention and their captivity
had not been subjected to any
meaningful review… further it
became more and more clear
many of the men were innocent,
or at a minimum their guilt
was impossible to determine
let alone prove in any court
of law, civilian or military…
the primary issue was to gain
more intelligence as quickly
as possible on Al Qaeda…
Sweeping up and detaining
large numbers of people… even
innocent people… was deemed
acceptable if it led to a more
complete and satisfactory
intelligence picture with
regard to Iraq, thus
justifying the
Administration's plans for war
with that country (Cited in
Leopold 2010, no page number).
Further as McCoy (2006b)
accounts:
Most of those
rounded up by military sweeps
in Iraq and Afghanistan for
imprisonment at Abu Ghraib and
Guantánamo had nothing to do
with terrorism. An analysis of
the Pentagon listing of
Guantánamo's 517 detainees
reveals that 86 percent were
arrested not by U.S. forces
but by Northern Alliance and
Pakistani warlords eager to
collect a $5,000 bounty for
every "terrorist" captured (no
page number).
In
closing this portion of our
focus group interviewees, we
reiterated the reality that the
vast majority of persons
initially swept in the war on
terror were not terrorists.
Further, we provided evidence
(Hope, Winnett, Watt and Blake,
2011) that suggested many were
in fact innocent persons who
were probably exposed to similar
interrogation techniques
described here. Student
reactions were often mixed and
many were skeptical regarding
this information, but this
questioning would always lead to
a discussion about the perceived
effectiveness of torture—and
this is where we turn next.
Theme Four:
Belief in the Efficacy of
Music Torture
In our debates about the utility
of torture, the majority of our
student-interviewees argued that
its purpose was to extract
crucial information that will
save lives and avert future
terrorist attacks. Even though
the majority of students
surveyed did not perceive music
(when used in this way) as
torture, they nonetheless had
the impression that its
application was a legitimate and
effective way to gain
information.
9/11 changed
everything… this was
unprecedented in human
history…they changed the
rules. So I it [music as a
component of interrogation] is
fair and legitimate … I think
this can really make someone
break and give up important
info (Student focus group
members, Spring 2008).
It's a
dangerous world and sometimes we
need to [do this] stuff to
protect us… if it gives us
information to save lives then
yes, we should be doing it
(Student focus group members,
Spring 2011).
I have no
doubt it works. If I were
being tortured I would talk.
I'd tell them everything I
know (Student focus group
member, Spring 2012).
While
we where there primarily to
query music students on their
awareness of the implementation
of music as a tool in the "war
on terror," we nonetheless felt
it necessary to engage such
debate and offer a contrasting
point of view when the efficacy
of torture was professed.
Specifically, we noted that
despite claims of effectiveness,
research often tells a different
tale. Mayer (2008:134), for
instance, argues:
Virtually
every expert in and outside
the government agrees…that
torture and lesser forms of
physical coercion will make
people confess something- but
the 'truth' is another matter.
Indeed,
in her work on the torture of
detainees in the global war on
terror, Mayer finds that nearly
every individual whom
experienced physical or
psychological brutality lied and
fabricated information to avoid
torture. According to other
research, intelligence
officials, detainees who endured
varying degrees of force did
tell their interrogators some
truths, but the majority told
half-truths and outright lies
(Alexander, Bruning, and Bowden
2011). Thus, the acceptance of
torture as an intelligence
gathering technique seems
fundamentally flawed. Even if
the detainee begins to talk
under torture, interrogators
have a hard time determining
whether he is telling the truth
or not. As Joe Navarro, a former
FBI agent and author of Advanced
Interviewing Techniques, writes
"torture only guarantees pain;
it never guarantees the truth"
(cited in Pesca 2007 no page
number).
Invariability students would
then ask something similar to
the following: But if torture
doesn't work, what does? Here we
would offer commentary from
experts in the field. Please
consider for example, commentary
from Navarro:
There is no
professional interviewer that
subscribes to torture or to
advanced or enhanced
interrogation techniques to
obtain information. It just
doesn't work…because the
purpose of an interview is to
get cooperation, not
compliance. You know, the FBI
has been interviewing
terrorists since the
anarchists back in the 1920s
and '30s. We've never had to
use any of these techniques.
And, in fact, none of the
intelligence services have
really ever asked for this.
9/11 didn't change the human
brain, and what we know is
that people will say anything
when they're being tortured…
traditional techniques work
just fine...techniques that
use rapport-based
techniques….that
psychologically seduce
individuals into cooperating
and providing information
(cited in Martin, 2013 no page
number).
Ultimately, experts say that
diligent intelligence sleuthing
and traditional interrogation
techniques are more effective in
gaining information than
torture. This said, why are
students so adamant in their
belief of the efficacy of
torture? As noted in earlier
sections, the portrayal of
torture in television and pop
culture, as well as in
statements issued by
governmental officials may have
contributed to the belief that
brutal interrogations are
necessary and effective.
Lastly,
despite several students' claims
that the brutality of 9/11 was
unprecedented in human history
(thus making the torture of "our
enemies" legitimate and
effective in extracting useful
information quickly), we often
closed this portion of the
discussion with the following
quotes for continued reflection.
Eighteen hundred years ago, the
Roman jurist named Ulpian, who
critiqued of the use of torture
in the Roman legal system,
stated: "Torture is a difficult
and deceptive thing for the
strong will resist and the weak
will say anything to end the
pain." Even a senior American
psych-ops instructor joked that
"if he were made to listen to
[repetitive music] he would
confess to being a member of
al-Quaeda [after] an hour [and]
after ten hours he'd confess to
being Osama bin Laden" (Pieslak,
2007:133).
Theme Five: Alters One's
Relationship to Music
Finally, as music is
unquestionably a powerful form
of human expression, we asked
students to describe their
personal relationship to music
and their feelings while playing
or creating music. Many
responded in ways similar to
those expressed below:
I love it
[making music]… I wish I could
do it all the time. It's an
escape from the bad things
that happen in life
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2008).
If I didn't
have music in my life, I think
I'd go crazy (Student focus
group member, Spring 2011).
Music is my
life. It brings me such joy
and whenever I create a new
work it's like I am creating
life… (Student focus group
member, Spring 2013).
Following this discussion, and
as a way to bring the discussion
back to potential role of music
as torture, we asked music
students how they would feel if
the music they had created was
used to inflict pain on another
person. While some comments were
snarky (one student for
example said: "As long as I get
paid, I don't care what they do
with my music), most were
introspective and seemed
emotively moved by this
question.
Students made the following
statements:
Ahhh… well I
think this would be
particularly perverted because
it takes something that's
meant to be enjoyable and
turns it into an instrument of
pain… this would strip the
prisoners of the future joy in
music – how can one enjoy it
after that (Student focus
group member, Spring 2013).
I suppose this
would make one "allergic" to
music. I couldn't imagine
anything more terrible… to be
robbed of the joy (Student
focus group member, Spring
2013).
Up till now
this whole conservation was a
joke to me, but I would hate
to think that my music was
[hurting] someone
(Student focus group member,
Spring 2011).
I write music
with God in my heart. The idea
that my music would be viewed
by [the detainee] as evil… I
know I couldn't do anything
about it, but I would feel
complicit (Student focus group
member, Spring 2012).
Ouch, I'd
never thought of this before.
I suppose I'd be angry that
something I created with joy
in my heart was used in a way
I didn't approve (Student
focus group member, Spring
2013).
Cumulatively, these
phenomenological expressions
suggest a change in the thought
processes associated with the
potential experience of music.
Previously, music students were
quick to highlight their vision
of music as a purely joyful
enterprise. Now however, music
was also to be conceived as an
activity that could produce a
negative experience for the
creator – namely that the
producer might be complicit in
negating the joy of music for
another. Imagining music and the
musician in this light was the
first time many viewed the
potential harm of forced
listening. At the end of these
particular discussions, the
researchers often noted how
students made their first
notable empathetic connections
to persons subjected to "music
torture"—and if nothing else, we
saw how a critical debate on
music, war, and its effects
might be engaged.
Discussion
and Conclusion
So what exactly, have we gained
from this research? Ultimately
this paper had a two-fold
purpose. The first was to
identify and review the use of
music as a technique of
interrogation and as a vehicle
of forced compliance on
prisoners in the "global war on
terror." We find that music
defies easy (legal)
classification as ''torture,"
even though prolonged exposure
to noise can produce extreme
psychological distress as it
deprives prisoners of rest and
sleep. In terms of physical
harm, loudness damages hearing
with prolonged exposure and may
reduce bodily functions, alter
intellectual capacities, and
produce disorientation, anxiety,
and fear. Truly, the pulses of
music and noise when used in
this manner blur the threshold
between physical and
psychological effects—and this
use and effect of "music as
torture" should continue to be
identified and studied. For
authorities that subscribe to
the use of torture, but want to
avoid criticism from the public,
music may provide a convenient
loophole. While the use of music
in interrogation practices
certainly feels like torture to
detainees, the public may merely
shrug off such practices as an
annoyance—which takes us to the
second part of our work.
The
second part of this paper was a
replication and extension of
earlier research by Cusick
(2006). Whereas Cusick initially
queried and recorded music
bloggers' perceptions of the
idea of music as torture, we
asked a very specific segment of
the public (music students at a
Midwestern American University)
if they are aware of these
practices and what they make of
music being used in these ways.
In general, the music students
surveyed were not overwhelmingly
aware of the use and role of
music as a tool of
interrogation. This suggests,
per the concern of music
scholars and teachers (as noted
by Bohlman, 2007 and Papeti and
Grant, 2013), that students may
be lacking in critical ethical
education about the
unconventional and controversial
uses of music—and if nothing
else, should be considered for
additional inclusion in their
curriculum.
When
students were made aware of such
modes of interrogation, most
were not inclined to define it
as torture because it is
unlikely to produce outwardly
visible marks of bodily damage.
Additionally, the idea of music
as torture was initially
trivialized and viewed with
humor—it was difficult for
students to view the application
of music as little more than a
nuisance when used on detainees.
Beyond this, music students
envisioned harsh or aggressive
interrogation tactics as
justifiable because they
conceived the detainees as
legitimate perpetrators of
terrorism. Many of our students
also believe in the
effectiveness of torture and
defended it because they say it
is needed in a world as
dangerous as this one—and while
this is beyond the scope of our
paper, these findings also point
towards a general public
passivity regarding revelations
of torture and acceptance of
potential war crimes. Indeed,
though these apathetic
orientations have been studied
elsewhere (Berinsky 2007,
Steuter and Wills 2008, Leitz
2011), the perceptions of our
music students, suggest that
additional study into the
influence of media, xenophobia,
and social power on war
trivialization is
warranted.
However, when we asked music
students how they would feel if
the music they had created was
used to inflict pain on another
person, attitudes changed
dramatically. For most of the
students, the creation of music
is a powerful form of expression
and an emotion akin to deep joy.
With the thought of their music
being used in ways alien to them
and without their permission,
many spoke in descriptors citing
anger, sadness, and sickness.
The idea that music as torture
might make people "allergic" to
ever enjoying music again was
something the majority had never
conceived —and this reality was
the most dismaying and
disturbing aspect of this
practice for our students.
As the
"war on terror" has turned music
into a potentially repulsive
experience for those engaged in
its application as interrogation
and torture, and we must now
conceive of song and sound as a
physical, psychological, and
sociopolitical tool for pain and
terror—and as such, we are also
forced to consider new subfields
within musicology and the
sociology of music. These being
potentially: soundscapes of pain
and the socio-musicology of
violence.
Further, while we held
conversational forums with music
students to identify patterns of
thought across a particular
student population, future
research should obviously
continue to collect narratives
from "music torture survivors."
Future research should also
assemble the impressions of
musicians whose songs were used
in such controversial ways. We
imagine that the insights
collected from both survivors
and musicians would offer a
"thick description" (Geertz,
1973) that goes beyond the first
reflections of music students
who were initially disconnected
from this reality. Moreover,
investigators should also gather
the experiences, opinions, and
beliefs of those soldiers and
political/military officials who
were directly involved in the
organization and application of
music as a tool in the "war on
terror." Certainly the absence
of these voices marks a
limitation of this paper. Yet,
these limitations also sketch a
direction for future research
and we hope that our paper, if
nothing else, helps illuminate
an important aspect of this is
often overlooked reality of
music, violence, and conflict.
In the end, the physical and
psychological damage caused by
music torture must be recognized
as a destructive force and we
scholars and critics should work
to broaden the understanding how
noise assaults bodies and may be
used as a tool of submission and
compliance. As Grant
(2013:13) writes:
If we are to
build a convincing case for
taking this issue seriously we
need to gather more
information and we need to
make public the scale of the
problem. This is where we are
so dependent on [scholars],
readers, and the wider
community of all engaged in
the fight against torture and
ill treatment.
References
Albert, Suart and
Geoffery G. Bell. 2002. Timing
and Music. The Academy of
Management Review, Vol.
27: 574-593.
Alexander, Matthew, John R
Bruning and Mark Bowden. 2011.
How to Break a
Terrorist: The U.S.
Interrogators Who Used
Brains, Not Brutality, to
Take Down the Deadliest Man
in Iraq. New York, NY:
St. Martin's Press.
Ammerman, Nancy. 1993.
Report to the Justice and
Treasury
Departments
Regarding Law Enforcement
Interaction With The Branch
Davidians in
Waco,
Texas. Available
Online:
http://digilib.bu.edu/dspace/bitstream/handle/2144/14
/ReportOnBranchDavidians.html?sequence=1
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
BBC News. 2003. "Sesame Street
breaks Iraqi POWs" Tuesday, 20
May.
Available Online:
http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/middle_east/3042907.stm
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
Benjamin, Mark. 2007. "The
CIA's Favorite Form of
Torture." Salon.
Available
Online:
http://www.salon.com/2007/06/07/sensory_deprivation/
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
Berinsky, Adam. 2007.
"Assuming the Costs of War:
Events, Elites, and
American
Public Support for Military
Conflict." Journal of
Politics 69:
975- 997.
Bohlman, Phillip V. 2007.
"Becoming Ethnomusicologists."
SEM Newsletter 41:1.
Cloonan, Martin and
Bruce Johnson. 2002.
"Killing me Softly with his
Song: An Initial
Investigation into the Use of
Popular Music as a Tool of
Oppression." Popular Music,
21: 27-39.
Cusick, Suzanne. 2006. "Music
as torture / Music as weapon."
Revista
Transcultural de
Música/Transcultural Music
Review. Available
Online:
http://www.sibetrans.com/trans/trans10/cusick_eng.htm
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
Cusick, Suzanne. 2008. 'You
are in a place that is out of
the world . . .': Music in the
Detention Camps of the 'Global
War on Terror'. Journal of
the Society for American
Music 2/1: 1-26.
Cusick, Suzanne and Branden
Joseph. 2011. "Across an
Invisible Line: A Conversation
about Music and Torture." Grey
Room 42: 6–21.
Glaser, Barney G &
Strauss, Anselm L., 1967. The
Discovery of Grounded
Theory: Strategies for
Qualitative Research.
Chicago, Aldine Publishing
Company.
Geertz, Clifford. 1973. The
Interpretation of Cultures:
Selected essays. New
York: Basic Books.
Grant, M.J. 2013. "The
Illogical Logic of Music
Torture" Torture Journal
23:4-13
Hogan, Mark 2013. "Metallica
Urged U.S. to Stop Using Their
Music for Torture, Says
SEAL Who Killed Bin Laden:
It's 'Kill 'Em All,' not
'Enhanced Interrogation 'Em
All'" Spin. Available
online:
http://www.spin.com/articles/metallica-torture-navy-seal-
osama-bin-laden-shooter/
(Last viewed August 11, 2013).
Hope, Christopher, Robert
Winnett, Holly Watt and Heidi
Blake. 2011.
"WikiLeaks:
Guantanamo Bay terrorist
secrets revealed." The
Telegraph
Available online:
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/worldnews/wikileaks/
8471907/WikiLeaks
Guantanamo-Bay-terrorist-secrets-revealed.html
(Last Viewed August 19,
2013).
Human Rights First (no date).
"Torture on TV Rising and
Copied in the Field."
Available Online:
http://www.humanrightsfirst.org/our-work/law-and-security/
torture-on-tv/
(Last visited August 15,
2013).
Human Rights Watch report
2006, "'No blood, No foul':
Soldiers' Accounts of
Detainee Abuse in Iraq."
Available Online:
http://www.hrw.org/reports/2006/07/22/no-blood-no-foul
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
Isakson, Brian L. and Gregory
J. Jurkovic. 2013. "Healing
After Torture: The Role of
Moving On." Qualitative
Health Research 23:749-761.
Krueger Richard A., and Casey
Mary Anne. 2009. Focus
Groups: A Practical
Guide for
Applied Research. 4th
ed. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Leitz, Lisa. 2011.
"Oppositional Identities: The
Military Peace Movement's
Challenge
to Pro-Iraq War Frames." Social
Problems 58:235-256.
Leopold, Jason. 2010. "Ex-Bush
Official Willing to Testify
Bush, Cheney Knew Gitmo
Prisoners
Innocent." Truthout.
Available Online:
http://www.truth-out.org/article/item/713:
exbush-official-willing-to-testify-bush-cheney
(Last viewed, August 8, 2013).
Mackey, Robert. 2009.
"Claim U.S. Used Eminem Raps
on Detainees." New York
Times: The Lede.
Available Online:
http://thelede.blogs.nytimes.com/2009/03/09/
claim-us-used-eminem-raps-
on-detainees/
(Last viewed, August 8, 2013).
Martin, Michel. 2013.
"Interrogations Without
Torture." NPR.
Available online at:
http://www.npr.org/2013/05/02/180570052/
interrogations-without-torture
(Last viewed August 19,
2013).
Mazzetti, Mark and William
Glaberson. 2009. "Obama Issues
Directive to Shut Down
Guantánamo." New
York Times, January 21.
Available Online:
http://www.nytimes.com/2009/01/22/us/politics/22gitmo.html
(Last viewed August 8, 2013).
Mayer, Jane. 2008. The Dark
Side. New York , NY:
Doubleday Press.
McCoy, Alfred W. 2006a. A
Question of Torture: CIA
Interrogation, from the Cold
War to the War on Terror.
New York: Metropolitan Books.
McCoy, Alfred W. 2006b. "The
Myth of the Ticking Time
Bomb." The Progressive.
Available
online:
http://progressive.org/mag_mccoy1006
(Last viewed August 19, 2013).
Newport, Kenneth G. C. 2006. The
Branch Davidians of Waco:
The History and
Beliefs of an
Apocalyptic Sect. Oxford
University Press.
Papeti, Anna and M.J. Grant.
2013. "Introduction" Torture
Journal 23:1-3.
Peisner, David. 2006. "Music
As Torture: War Is Loud." Spin.
Available online:
http://www.spin.com/articles/music-torture-war-loud/
(Last Viewed August 11,
2013).
Pesca, Mike. 2007. "Why do
People Torture." NPR Available
online at:
http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=10046000
(Last viewed August 18, 2013).
Physicians for Human Rights.
2010. "Sensory Bombardment." PHR
Toolkits.
Available online:
http://phrtoolkits.org/toolkits/
istanbul-protocol-model-medical-curriculum/
module-4-torture-methods-and-their-medical-consequences/
torture-methods
/sensory-bombardment/
(Last viewed August 11,
2013).
Pieslak, Jonathan R. 2007.
"Sound Targets: Music and the
War in Iraq." Journal
of Musicological
Research 26:123 – 149.
Piore, Adam. 2003. "Psyop: The
Love's Not Mutual." Newsweek,
May 26:13.
Potter, R.A. 1998. "Noise,
Performance, and the Politics
of Sound." Pp. 31-46 in T.
Swiss, J. Sloop and A. Herman
(eds.), Mapping the Beat.
Oxford University
Press.
Rouse, Ed. 2000. Psyop in
Panama: Operation Just Cause.
[Available:
www.psywarrior.com].
Scheppele, Kim L. 2005.
"Hypothetical Torture in the
War on Terrorism," Journal
of National Security Law
& Policy 1: 285–340.
Smith, Clive. 2008. "Welcome
to 'the disco.'" The
Guardian, Thursday June
19
Available Online:
http://www.guardian.co.uk/world/2008/jun/19/usa.guantanamo
(Last Viewed, August 8, 2013).
Steuter, Erin and Deborah
Wills. 2008. At War with
Metaphor: Media, Propaganda,
and Racism in the War on
Terror. Boulder,
Colorado: Lexington Books.
Strauss, Anselm L., and Juliet
M Corbin. 1990. Basics of
Qualitative Research:
Grounded Theory Procedures
and Techniques. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage
Publications, Inc.
Strauss Anselm L. and Juliet
M.Corbin. 1997. Grounded
Theory in Practice.
Newbury
Park, CA: Sage Publications,
Inc.
US Department of Defense.
2005. "Secretary Rumsfeld
Interview with Jerry Agar KMBZ
News Radio 980 Kansas City,
Kan." Transcript, Available
Online:
http://www.defense.gov/Transcrip/Transcript.aspx?TranscriptID=3246
(Last Viewed August 11,
2013).
Worthington, Andy. 2008. "A
History of Music Torture in
the 'War on Terror.'"
Andyworthington.co.uk.
Available online:
http://www.andyworthington.co.uk/
2008/12/15/a-history-of-music-torture-in-the-war-on-terror/
(Last visited August 8, 2013).
Appendix:
Opening Vignettes and
Guiding Questions
*It's been
reported that the US Army has
used music on Iraqi and Afghani
prisoners to break their
resistance to interrogation,
"through sleep deprivation,
disorientation, and playing
music that is culturally
offensive to them" (Source: BBC
News. 2003. "Sesame Street
breaks Iraqi POWs" Tuesday, 20
May).
*In some cases
prisoners have been held in
garage-sized, windowless spaces
where songs like Metallica's
"Enter Sandman," Barney the
Purple Dinosaur's "I Love You,"
and Decide's "Fuck Your God"
have been played over a
loudspeaker for days on end. In
one particular case detainees
were made to listen to a daily
repeated loop of crying babies
mixed with the Meow Mix cat food
commercial (Source: Pieslak,
Jonathan R. 2007. "Sound
Targets: Music and the War in
Iraq." Journal of Musicological
Research, 26: 123 – 149).
Guiding
Questions:
What are your
general reactions to this?
Does it upset
you to learn that music is used
this way? Why or Why not?
What would be
your reaction if you learned
that a song "you loved" was used
in ways similar to those
described in the opening
vignette? Would it change the
emotional experience you have
with the song?
What would be
your reaction if you learned
that an artist or band "you
loved" endorsed (gave approval)
to their music being used to
"soften" detainees prior to
interrogation?
What would be
your reaction if you learned
that music you had created was
being used in ways similar to
those described in the opening
vignette?
Do you perceive
music, when used in this way, as
torture? In what ways do you
consider it to be torture—In
what ways is it not?
Finally, in your
opinion what is the role or
purpose of music in society?
©
2015 Sociation Today
A Member of the EBSCO Publishing Group
Abstracted in Sociological Abstracts
Online
Indexing and Article Search from the
Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ)
Return to Home
Page to Read More Articles
Sociation Today is
optimized for the Firefox Browser
The Editorial Board of Sociation Today
Editorial Board:
Editor:
George H. Conklin,
North Carolina
Central University
Emeritus
Robert Wortham,
Associate Editor,
North Carolina
Central University
Board:
Rebecca Adams,
UNC-Greensboro
Bob Davis,
North Carolina
Agricultural and
Technical State
University
Catherine Harris,
Wake Forest
University
Ella Keller,
Fayetteville
State University
Ken Land,
Duke University
Steve McNamee,
UNC-Wilmington
Miles Simpson,
North Carolina
Central University
William Smith,
N.C. State University
|
|