The
stereotype of the Mrs. Degree alleges
that some women attending college
choose an "easy" or "less useful"
major, often identified as those
majors with a less explicit career
path, under the assumption that they
will not need to use this degree in a
career. Instead, these women use their
college years to find a husband, whom
they expect to marry shortly after
graduation, so that they can be
housewives. This stereotype emerged in
the 1950s when women started attending
college in larger numbers, and it did
develop out of an actual phenomenon.
However, in the wake of second-wave
feminism and the women's movement of
the 1970s, women have started moving
away from these limited domestic roles
of wife, mother, and homemaker,
holding professional careers and
marrying at much later ages. It is
troubling, then, that this stereotype
lingers, often uttered as a criticism
of certain female-dominated majors and
the female students whose goals are
suspected to tend towards domesticity,
which is seen as a lack of ambition
among other college students. Why does
this stereotype linger? Given the
changing gender composition of
universities and within certain
undergraduate majors and the shifting
marriage patterns, is there a
practical basis for the Mrs. Degree,
or is it merely an example of sexism
in higher education that stems from a
suspicion of female ambition? To test
this question, I hypothesize that, if
the Mrs. Degree stereotype plays out
on today's college campuses, then
women in "less useful majors," such as
traditionally female majors like those
in the humanities, are more likely to
be married and less likely to be
employed than those in the "useful"
(and traditionally masculine) majors,
such as those in business and health
fields.
Literature Review
The
stereotype of the Mrs. Degree has been
a pervasive challenge towards women
pursuing a college degree since they
were granted wider access to secondary
education. The trope represented a
woman who attended college or
university for the sole purpose of
finding a husband and got married as
soon as possible, sometimes before
graduation. This image emerged from a
trend in the 1950s which Judy Blume
recollects from her personal
experience, in which "you go to
college; you get a degree in education
in case, God forbid, you ever have to
go to work, and while you're there,
you'd damn well better meet the man
you're going to marry because
otherwise, where else are you going to
meet him?" ("Part One...") This
mindset revealed women's limited
independence in the post-war era in
which this trend emerged. It was,
quite simply, a practical solution to
ensure personal security. One
researcher hypothesizes that women in
the 1950s considered this Mrs. Degree
as a smart financial move because
attending college provided a created a
strong opportunity of finding and
"marrying a college-educated husband
with high earnings potential"
(Jacobs). However, this path assumes
the woman will remain in the domestic
sphere, as it provides limited career
plans beyond the role of housewife,
mother and homemaker. As Blume further
recollects, it even created a toxic
environment for women because this
opportunity for education was reduced
to a race to the altar, in which
engaged victors "would raise their
hand…so you could see the diamond"
because "that's what it was all about:
the ring, the silver pattern, the
china" and all the other trappings of
married domesticity ("Part One…").
Indeed, this pattern resulted in a
post-WWII surge in exceptionally young
marriages. In this time, the
aforementioned description was not an
uncommon illustration of the female
college experience; in fact, at that
time, there was a strong correlation
between higher levels of educational
achievement and likelihood of
marrying, especially at a young age
(Glick and Carter).
This positive relationship between
college education and likelihood of
marriage continues to hold true. In
fact, women with college degrees are
more likely to get married than those
without degrees in rates higher than
the so-called "baby boomer" post-WWII
cohorts (Goldstein and Kenney).
However, recent trends show a shift in
marriage rates in which women,
especially college-educated women, are
getting married later than the baby
boomer cohorts did. It is very
important to note that this change
represents merely a widespread
tendency to delay marriage rather than
a shift towards non-marriage, due in
part to the rise in cohabitation among
dating couples as a precursor to
marriage and the presumable tendency
for women to seek work after college
graduation rather than marrying right
away (Oppenheimer, Goldstein and
Kenney). Employment does not decrease
the likelihood of a woman getting
married; depending on the rigidity of
gender roles, employed women might be
more likely to get married (Ono,
Oppenheimer). Indeed, many married
women continue working (Oppenheimer,
Glick and Carter, Goldstein and
Kenney).
In spite of these changes, the Mrs.
Degree trope persists, but now it is
the idea that certain majors are
inherently less useful in the
workforce than others, leading to the
conclusion that a woman must have
selected her particular major as a
filler with which to bide her time
until she gets married. This attitude
can be attributed to the degree with
which a young woman holds traditional
gender roles, which are developed
during socialization from the family,
specifically by parental views towards
women in higher education and
marriage. Research shows that parents'
socioeconomic status (which can be
captured by education levels or
occupation) determines parents'
attitudes toward gender roles, and it
is a statistically significant
indication of these views being passed
on to their children (Cunningham).
Beyond socialization, the basis of
this view is understandable, given the
breadth of research that has observed
a gender divide in major choices,
despite the higher number of women
receiving undergraduate degrees
(England and Li, Jacobs).
Generally speaking, women are
underrepresented in engineering and
technical sciences—particularly
computer science—and they are more
likely to change majors if they
initially declared majors in one of
these fields (Turner and Brown,
England and Li, Dickson). Instead,
they are more inclined to select
majors in the humanities, especially
English and education, or social
sciences such as sociology and
psychology. Increasingly, women are
majoring in physical and life
sciences, particularly biology
(Dickson, England and Li, Ma). Turner
and Bowen suggest that the gender
divide within the sciences might have
to do with math intensity, in which
men are more likely to use more
math-oriented disciplines (1999). Ma
observes that women are more drawn
towards majors in "helping"
professions because of the intrinsic,
altruistic and social rewards of a
career than men, who are more
concerned with finding jobs with more
money and prestige. However, this
divide only holds true for students of
high socioeconomic status (2009).
Research has not considered students'
outcomes in given majors, even though
the broad gender lines affect their
success in the workforce (Ma). Bose
and Whaley describe this phenomenon of
"pink-collar occupations," which are
female-dominated white-collar
occupations, sometimes labeled as
semi-skilled instead of fully
professional jobs because they are
less prestigious, pay lower salaries,
and have less autonomy for employees
(2001). Men are less likely to enter
these fields, but those who do advance
much quicker than women, while women
in male-dominated fields earn less,
showing the systemic tendency to
devalue women's work (Bose and
Whaley). They further argue that this
trend can be explained by human
capital theory, defined as
"characteristics or skills that make a
worker more productive and attractive
to an employer," with women taking
paths providing less human capital
with "the resulting female-dominated
occupations [being] low-skilled,
hav[ing] short career ladders, be[ing]
amenable to intermittent
employment…based on a 1950s
understanding of women's roles" (Bose
and Whaley). Given the gendered
perception of these majors—there is no
male equivalent of the Mrs. Degree
stereotype; a man is expected to earn
a degree to get a job—it is worth
examining whether or not there is any
truth to this concept. This imbalance
raises further questions of how men in
female-dominated majors' employment
rates compare with their female
counterparts.
Methods
To
test for the existence of the Mrs.
Degree, I used data from the 2012 and
2014 General Social Survey (GSS). The
GSS is a sociological survey conducted
every other year by the National
Opinion Research Center (NORC) that
samples English and Spanish-speaking
adults over the age of 18 to measure
the demographic characteristics and
attitudes of American citizens. For
this research, I selected recent
college graduates between ages 25 and
35 from all GSS respondents in the
selected survey years to serve as my
sample. This sample reflects current
trends of marriage patterns and
college major choice while also
measuring instances of younger
marriage. Including a wider age range
in the sample would introduce the
possibility of confounding results
stemming from incidents occurring
later in life, such as divorces, death
of a spouse, and second marriages. I
used crosstables to analyze the
relationships between the following
variables:
- Undergraduate
major choice (Major1;
independent)—this variable is
important because post-graduate
degrees are less important to the
purposes of this research, as the
Mrs. Degree stereotype does not hold
as strongly after the undergraduate
degree. Presumably, those students
who wish to go to graduate programs
are serious about their academic and
career pursuits. For ease of
interpretation, I recoded the
original 80-plus responses of
college majors into nine categories:
business, communications, health,
social sciences, physical sciences
(such as biology and chemistry),
technical sciences (such as computer
science and engineering),
humanities, arts, and other. The
design of the hypotheses meant that
I focused on three of these
categories: health and business (the
male-dominated majors) and
humanities (the female-dominated
major).
- Marital
Status (Marital; dependent)—for ease
of interpretation this variable was
also recoded into three categories:
married, never married, or other.
Given the age of the sample
respondents and the research
question, divorce and death of
spouse are less important
considerations.
- Workforce
Status (wrkstat; dependent)—this
variable measures respondents'
employment status, with multiple
detailed categories that were very
helpful to the purposes of this
research. Besides listing working
full- or part-time and unemployment,
respondents could also select
"keeping house," a category that is
crucial to the research question, as
it specifically addresses the
possibility of married women filling
the role of homemaker.
- Sex
(control)—to show gendered
differences in post-graduate paths
as indicated by the literature.
Findings
Generally, I found that the data did
not support the hypothesis proposed by
the Mrs. Degree stereotype, meaning
that the Mrs. Degree stereotype is not
replicated in contemporary American
society. Even without considering
college major choice, the percentage
of married women working (58.4%) is
greater than the percentage of married
women keeping house (35.5%), which
indicates, at least in broad strokes,
a pattern of working women that does
not indicate support for the Mrs.
Degree, even though this model
includes women who did not attend
college and are therefore are exempt
from the Mrs. Degree stereotype (Table
1). Furthermore, at 43.6%, the
percentage of married women working
full-time is greater than married
women keeping house. At least on the
macro-level, the phenomenon of women
almost exclusively filling the
domestic roles of mother, wife, and
homemaker is not supported by the
data. This finding is a notable first
step towards disproving the Mrs.
Degree stereotype because it marks a
movement away from the 1950s
understanding of gender roles that
fostered the development of the
stereotype in the first place.
Although this data does reflect the
general trend of larger numbers of
working women than in previous
generations, it is important to
remember that it is a measure of all
women between ages 25 and 35, so the
sample includes women who are not
college graduates. This model is
useful from a foundational standpoint
and offers the hope that similarly
encouraging results will emerge for
female college graduates, regardless
of their major. (see Table 1)
However, it is important to note
that the first model does not mark
the complete overthrow of
traditional gender roles, as
illustrated when considering the
same general trends for young men.
Just over 90% of married men
between the ages 25 and 35 are
working, with 86.1% working
full-time (Table 2). This huge gap
between married men and women
working full-time indicates that
men remain, in many cases, the
primary breadwinner of the
household.
With less than 1% of
married men in the sample keeping
house instead of working, the idea of
stay-at-home dads, a possibility
raised by the trend of larger numbers
of working mothers, is not one that is
reflected in this data. (See Table 2).
There is no inversion of gender role
expectations when it comes to which
partner is expected to be the primary
one who fills roles in the domestic
sphere; homemaking remains an almost
exclusively female space. A noticeably
larger percentage of men indicated
that they are unemployed or
temporarily not working (6.2%).
Although there is no way to measure
how long they have been unemployed, it
is interesting to consider the
possibility that these men may
consider their situation to be a
temporary one, even if they have
potentially been out of work for a
significant period of time. Even if
the male partner is the one at home
more, in the absence of full-time
employment, he does not consider
homemaking to be a permanent role.
However, this pattern does not
completely cancel out the encouraging
trend of more women in the workplace,
especially because the same general
findings are largely replicated when
measuring college major. The
relatively high levels of employment
across majors for female college
graduates at least partially disproves
the Mrs. Degree stereotype because
marriage and homemaking are not used
as alternatives to pursuing a career.
Overall, 77.6% of female college
graduates are employed, with 62%
working full-time, which are strong
rates in a competitive job market like
the one we currently face (Table 3).
Interestingly, female humanities
majors—the category of majors most
closely associated with the Mrs.
Degree stereotype—had 80.7% of
respondents working full-time, second
only to female arts majors (81.1%), a
discipline that receives similar
suspicion of "usefulness" in the
workforce. The Mrs. Degree stereotype
would imply that these majors should
have the smallest percentages of women
working full-time. On the other hand,
women in the traditionally
male-dominated, "more useful" majors
do not have the highest percentages of
full-time employment, with 78.7% of
female business majors working
full-time and a markedly low 48.6% of
female health majors working
full-time. Very few majors have large
percentages of women working
part-time, which is an interesting
complication of the proposal that
women can balance work and homemaking
by holding a part-time job. However,
female health majors do have a
relatively high percentage (27.5%) of
respondents working part-time, which
could be potentially be explained by
the tendency for women to work in
greater numbers as nurses, a position
that can more easily be a part-time
job rather than the higher-prestige
healthcare occupations that are mostly
filled by men. It is very interesting
to note that, for most majors, the
percentage of women who are not
working and keeping house is generally
less than 20%. The most noticeable
exception to this finding is for
female technical science majors, of
which 32.6% are homemakers, but very
few women choose these majors, which
is reflected by an extremely small
number of 9 women in this category.
Therefore, this number is overinflated
by the very small sample size. In this
specific finding, gender differences
in college major are reflected in the
findings, but the relatively low gap
in employment levels between men and
women reflect the encouraging shift of
women working in larger numbers.
The results for male college
graduates' workforce status has very
interesting comparisons that further
fail to support the hypothesis,
illustrated in Table 4. Most
interesting among these is the finding
that humanities majors have the
largest relative percentage of male
college graduates keeping house
(8.2%). Although this highest male
level compares with the lowest levels
of women keeping house, this
particular measure is noteworthy
because a majority of the college
major categories do not report a
measure of male students keeping
house. This finding could reflect the
emergence of stay-at-home dads to
accommodate more working mothers as
the family's primary breadwinner, but
the similar levels of men keeping home
and men not working raises the
possibility of some overlap in these
categories. Perhaps some of these
stay-at-home fathers are performing
some identity work to reconcile their
current unemployment with traditional
male roles in the household as the
breadwinner in which they identify
their unemployment as a temporary
status instead of acknowledging their
long-term role as homemaker. For these
men, such an acknowledgement could be
conflated with a concession of defeat:
their failure at performing
traditional masculinity. This
conclusion is feasible when we
consider the fact that, for each
major, men have generally higher
percentages of full-time employment
compared to their female counterparts,
with three majors reporting measures
greater than 80%, roughly the highest
measure for women in any major. These
extremely elevated measures are
perhaps less surprising because two of
these measures are for majors that are
in traditionally male-dominated
fields: Business (91.8%) and Technical
Sciences (81.2%). The third
discipline, Communications, reports
93.4% of male majors holding full-time
employment, which is somewhat
surprising because it is not
necessarily a male-dominated field.
While this finding could be an
instance of the "glass escalator" in
which men in female-dominated fields
have greater opportunity for promotion
and more rapid advancement, there is
an extremely small number of male
Communications majors in this sample,
which limits the significance of this
particular measure.
The second portion of the hypothesis
concerning the relationship between
college major choice and marital
status also failed to support the
existence of the Mrs. Degree
stereotype. Although women in the
Humanities have among the higher
relative percentages of majors who are
married (66.7%), the overall range of
married respondents is relatively
narrow, with the lowest measure being
49% of female Physical Science majors
and the highest measure being 69.6% of
female Technical Science majors (Table
5). This finding implies that for any
given major, the possibility of these
women being married and working
full-time are not mutually exclusive
options, as the stereotype would
believe. In some cases, being
unmarried is an equally viable option
because for half of the majors, the
difference between married and never
married respondents is close to a
50/50 split, which certainly fails to
support the Mrs. Degree stereotype.
Quite interestingly, among these
majors are two female-dominated
disciplines: Communications, with
49.1% of female respondents reporting
they have never married, and Social
Sciences, with 41.1% of female
students never married. However, it is
important to note that overall, a
majority of female college graduates
(60.6%) are married. Women tend to
marry younger across the board,
regardless of college major choice.
Male college graduates do not have
such a pronounced split between
married and unmarried respondents. The
division is closer to an even split:
43.8% of male college graduates were
married and 54.3% had never married
(Table 6). This tendency towards an
even split is replicated across most
majors; the two significant exceptions
favor non-marriage, with 64.3% of male
Humanities majors being unmarried and
100% of male Arts majors being
unmarried. In the latter case, this
extreme finding is a result of the
extremely small sample size, with only
6 male Arts majors. Generally, these
findings support patterns that larger
portions of men marry at later ages
than women. It would appear that these
gendered differences in marriage
patterns are operating on a deeper,
more complex level than college major
choice, given the absence of a
distinct relationship between these
two variables for both men and women.
Conclusions
While speaking in broad terms, the
conditions defined by the Mrs. Degree
stereotype are not replicated in the
data, as no one major is more inclined
to have women with high rates of
marriage, low rates of employment, and
high rates of respondents serving as
the homemaker. However, it does appear
that the internalization of
traditional gender roles, those which
were at work when the stereotype
emerged in the 1950s, are still in
operation. Despite the relatively high
percentages of women working across
the board, especially for women
working full-time, these rates are
never as high as those for men,
implying that the male presence is
more expected in the workplace. The
disparity between married men and
women keeping house also contribute to
this narrative of devaluing women's
work. The distinct gap between
unemployed married women and married
women keeping house, with higher
levels of women keeping house,
indicates that there is no shame for a
woman to be the homemaker. After all,
there are elements in society that
continue imply that the domestic
sphere is her expected place, despite
the larger numbers of women taking the
professional workplace by storm. On
the other hand, the levels of
unemployed married men and married men
keeping house are very close to equal,
which raises the possibility of some
overlap in this sample. That is,
because the domestic sphere is
devalued as a feminine space, as are
many things which are associated with
women, it would be shameful for men to
admit that their wives are the primary
breadwinners in the couple. It would
be seen as a mark of loss of manliness
that the husband must fill the
"woman's role" while she "brings home
the bacon." To save face, these men
might see their unemployment as no
more than a temporary state, even if
they have been unemployed for some
time, rather than admit that they are
not fulfilling their duties to be the
"man of the house." In these ways,
gender norms are upheld, even if this
lingering example of sexism in higher
education is not replicated in
reality.
A major limitation of this study is
that the sample size is very small in
the models measuring college major
choice. These extremely small samples
limit the confidence with which we can
interpret these models'
generalizability. For majors that
composed particularly small
percentages of the population, this
problem is especially true, which
means that some of the control
conditions (women in male-dominated
majors and vice versa) can only be
interpreted with caution, as their
statistical significance is limited at
best. Further research with a larger
sample size would eliminate any errors
or false reports in the existing
models and allow for greater
generalizability of the results.
Bibliography
Bose,
C. E. & Whaley, R. B. (2001). Sex
Segregation in the US Labor Force.
Feminist Frontiers, 9th ed.
McGrath Hill: New York. 2009. Print.
Dickson,
Lisa. "Race and Gender Differences in
College Major Choice." The Annals of
the American Academy of Political and
Social Science 627 (2010):
108-124. JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
England,
Paula and Su Li. "Desegregation Stalled:
The Changing Gender Composition of
College Majors, 1971-2002." Gender
and Society 20.5 (2006): 657-677.
JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Glick,
Paul C. and Hugh Carter. "Marriage
Patterns and Educational Level." American
Sociological Review 23.3 (1958):
294-300. JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Goldstein,
Joshua R. and Catherine T. Kenney.
"Marriage Delayed or Marriage Forgone?
New Cohort Forecasts of First Marriage
for US Women." American Sociological
Review 66.4 (2001): 506-519.
JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Jacobs,
Jerry A. "Gender Inequality and Higher
Education." Annual Review of
Sociology. 22 (1996): 153-185.
JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Ma,
Yingyi. "Family Socioeconomic Status,
Parental Involvement, and College Major
Choices—Gender, Race/Ethnic, and
Nativity Patterns." Sociological
Perspectives 52.2 (2009):
211-234. JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Ono,
Hiromi. "Women's Economic Standing,
Marriage Timing, and Cross-National
Contexts of Gender." Journal of
Marriage and Family 65.2 (2003):
275-286. JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
Oppenheimer,
Valerie Kincade. "Women's Employment and
the Gain to Marriage: The Specialization
and Trading Model." Annual Review of
Sociology 23 (1997): 431-453.
JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.
"Part One: Awakening."
Makers: Women Who Make America. Prod.
Dyllan McGee and Rachel Dretzin. Public
Broadcasting Service (PBS), 2013.
Kanopy. Web. 18 Nov. 2015.
Turner,
Sarah E. and William G. Bowen. "Choice
of Major: The Changing (Unchanging)
Gender Gap." Industrial and Labor
Relations Review 52.2 (1999):
289-313. JSTOR. Web. 1 Dec. 2015.