The Official Journal of The North Carolina Sociological Association: A Peer-Reviewed Refereed Web-Based Publication ISSN 1542-6300 Editorial Board: Editor: George H. Conklin, North Carolina Central University Board: Rebecca Adams, UNC-Greensboro Bob Davis, North Carolina Agricultural and Technical State University Catherine Harris, Wake Forest University Ella Keller, Fayetteville State University Ken Land, Duke University Miles Simpson, North Carolina Central University Ron Wimberley, N.C. State University Robert Wortham, North Carolina Central University Editorial Assistants John W.M. Russell, Technical Consultant Austin W. Ashe, Duke University Submission
Cumulative
Sociation Today
The North
|
Volume 8, Number 1 Spring/Summer 2010 Does Culture Have Inertia?
by Shyamal Das
Lisa Eargle
Asraf Esmail
Inertia of Culture:
Inertia succumbs to "the resistance of a body at rest to being moved, or of a body in motion to being accelerated, decelerated, or redirected. Bodies are assumed to continue in their present state, whether at rest or in motion, without additional input of energy" (Schneider 2001:3). Applying this understanding to culture, culture behaves like a body, which could be accelerated towards adopting particular values; or it may decelerate or redirect towards earlier values. Let us further explain the idea using an example. A culture may assimilate sexual conservatism(1), by the followers of a particular culture (the body) either keeping conservatism for a relatively long period or they could accelerate towards full accentuation of sexual liberalism within the culture. The movement or static condition of a cultural component may depend on the social costs or benefits incurred by a social episode or event, which may greatly relate to the cultural component concerned. Therefore, it can be argued that any presumed "unintended" consequence(s) (e.g. HIV/AIDS) of any new cultural values and practices (e.g. sexual liberalism) may lead people to sustain or change their existing values. If this (these) "unintended" consequence(s) is (are) very costly (e.g. increase in mortality, growing dependency ratio etc.), the body (culture) would have inertia maintaining the pre-existing cultural component for a long time, while the benefits of the move would accelerate the body towards assimilating new values, and thereby resisting inertia. In the present paper, we argue that the spread of HIV/AIDS is the force or social episode that has contributed to the inertia of sexual conservatism in several societies, because HIV/AIDS is an episode that brought huge loss for the societies concerned. In other words, while the modernization of cultural values as a result of the globalization process has implanted sexual liberalism in many cultures around the world, sexual conservatism still prevails, because of the presumed negative effects of sexual liberalism (see Akoshah-Sarapong 2000; Kamwendo and Kamowa 1999; Ruel and Cambell 2006). Therefore, the cost (here, it is HIV/AIDS) for assimilating values related to sexual liberalization led the countries to maintain the sexual conservatism that had already existed. This is what we call "Inertia of Values or Attitudes." Thus, the cost exposed from a social episode has reverse effects on the corresponding values. The conceptual framework provided above has some complexities, however. We refer to two of them here. One may ask whether the items of particular values (e.g. sexual conservatism or liberalism) behave the same way when affected by a social episode. This is the most difficult part to answer. For example, the members of a society may continue denying homosexuality exists, yet they may want prostitution be in place. To explain this situation in terms of inertia and change, we argue that like a body, all components of a culture may not be in the same condition while facing a social episode. Therefore, an examination of the relationships between the social episode or cost and the items of the values concerned are required to reach a concrete conclusion. The second complexity is whether the social episode itself is fuelled by cultural values. We argue, following the law of inertia in body, that as the episode affects values, the initial values affect episodes as well. For example, the existence of extensive prostitution may lead to the outbreak of HIV/AIDS (see Hope Sr. 1999; Kaya 1999). Thus, on the one hand the existing beliefs and attitudes may enhance the episode, while the episode may promote inertia or change in values. However, the present paper examines only one hypothesis: controlling for other factors, HIV/AIDS contributes to cultural inertia towards sexual conservatism. The basic premise of our theory is this: if any social episode produces cost or a huge burden for society, the corresponding cultural values that are thought to be resistant to the cost would be sustained by the society for a long time; this condition is called the "inertia of cultural values". This is perhaps one of the reasons why the Iranian people opted for fundamentalism in 1979 after a huge injection of modern values in the society during Shah's regime. Or, this may explain why Islamic fundamentalism receives more support in recent years in Bangladesh, in comparison to the time when they fought against fundamentalism in 1971. Taking this view into account, the following section elaborates the relationship between a social episode and the inertia of culture. Social Episode or Event:
Referring to the theory of Sewell (1996), Moaddel (2007), in his recent excellent account on the relationship between historical events/ episodes and value changes among Islamic nations, shows that the significant historical events or episodes may contribute largely to the changes of the values in any society. The meaning of any episode impacts the society in a structured way, and this "[.] meaning determines whether and how individuals, and entire societies, reexamine their attitudes toward and beliefs about historically significant issues" (Moaddel 2007: 249). The terms "event(s) or episode(s)" can be used, as we argue, for outbreak of a disaster or disease in a society. While Moaddel (2007) puts reliance on several scholars (e.g. Collins 1998; Moore 1996; Wuthnow 1989) in explaining social disorganization or the reformation and articulation of the ideology structures that unleashes creativity as the results of events or episodes, none of these scholars examined whether these episodes or events even can lead to inertia instead of change in the cultural values and attitudes. While Moaddel (2007: 252) defines events or episodes as a "bounded historical process that has a beginning and an end", we argue that the "end" part of an event or episode may not be visible shortly after the "beginning" part starts. Therefore, an "event" or "episode" may capture a significant amount of time in taking full shape, and in the mean time, this event or episode may have a significant influence on the inertia or change in the value systems of society. While the events may influence change in values "[b]y causing ruptures in social structure, changing the balance of social forces or dramatically affecting human emotion" (Moaddel 2007: 252), these ruptures or other changes in social forces may also cause cultural inertia confirming the existing values and beliefs remaining the same for a long time. For example, the significant number of deaths caused by HIV/AIDS in Africa may lead their people to believing in the traditional sexual conservatism. This example suggests that the meaning of the collective trauma, "a socially mediated attribution" (Moaddel 2007: 257), spread by HIV/AIDS may cause the inertia of cultural values. The meaning incurred by any episode or event may hit on the emotions of the members of society (Pennebaker, Paez, and Rime 1997). We argue that the emotion of the people is commandeered by either sponsoring a new discourse or by reintroducing/reinforcing old social-ideological base to endorse either a new power-regime or a validating the old hegemonic control. The latter (old hegemonic control) may uphold the inertia of cultural values and attitudes. Simply put, a significant socio-historical episode may either give birth to the emergence of a new discourse to bring change in values (Moaddel 2005) or maintain the old order by enforcing old hegemonic discourses that endorse the inertia of values. Of course, any significant episode or event may be elevated by important socio-economic-political-cultural conditions. Combining the above and the work of post-modern scholars (e.g. Beck 1992 and 1994), it is noteworthy that any existing social discourse leading to increase risk in the society may cause the occurrence of a significant event or episode in society, which may lead to a change (e.g. moving to a "reflexive modernization"(2)or politico-ideological alteration) or an inertia in the value systems of the society. The social stigma attached to the episode or event may lead to change or inertia depending on the context. Let us give a very brief scrutiny of the social discourses that relate to sexual values and the cost attached to them. For example, homosexual practices, support for which is one of the components of sexual liberalism, were blamed for the spread of HIV/AIDS at one point of time, while heterosexuals in Africa have the highest rates of HIV/AIDS-affected people (Cameron 2001, and Ruxrungtham, Brown, and Phanuphak 2004 cited in Pick and Dayaram 2006). While the dominant social-discourse in Malawi proposes that the sexual liberalism (giving up traditional sexual beliefs) is responsible for the spread of HIV/AIDS (cited in Kamwedo and Kamowa 1999), this huge risk may lead people believing in and maintaining sexual conservatism, which confirms heterosexual dominance in society, and thus, the inertia of sexual values is upheld by the society. Let us cite a paragraph representing the discourse blaming sexual liberalism: Alas! With so-called modernity, and its attendant globalization, the African cultural atmosphere has been riped open by the uncontrolled thunder of modernity, opening the floodgate to premarital sex and children resulting from it. And what was considered a cultural anomaly has been thrown to the winds. The result is a continent whose very soul is under siege from HIV/AIDS pandemic" (Akoshah-Sarapong 2000).Several discourses blame western modernity for sexual values (e.g. the acceptance of homosexuality, abortion, cohabitation etc.) for the spread of HIV/AIDS (e.g. Islamic Prevention Versus Western Prevention 2007, available at (http://www.islamset.com/ bioethics/aids/versus.html). The Islamic-oriented discourses portray the situation as a clash of sexual orientations, which supplements Inglehart and Welzel's (2005) thesis of "half truth" to oppose Huntington's clash of civilization thesis (1996). Based on the above, the present research investigates whether the event itself, i.e., HIV/AIDS, caused change or inertia in sexual values. Data and Methods Data The population for this study is constructed
from the list of countries provided in World Values Surveys (2004). World
Values Surveys have conducted surveys on value systems of a total of 82
countries by the year 2004. We take all of our variables on attitudes and
values from these surveys.
Measures Independent Variables To evaluate the lagged effects of episode on the attitudes about sexual behavior, we use HIV/AIDS as our episode. To measure this, we take the percentage of population who has HIV/AIDS in the age group of 15 through 49. The obvious reason for taking this group into consideration is that this is the most vulnerable group the mode of sexuality of which relates to the society's productivity, including the leadership of the society itself. People are sexually most active in this age group. We used HIV/AIDS data for 1989 and 1995 allowing at least three years to influence peoples' attitude regarding sexuality in 1993-98 and 1999-2004 respectively. Moderators: Age, Gender, and Education Referring to other scholars (e.g. Alwin, Cohen, and Newcomb 1991; Schuman and Rodgers 2004), Moaddel (2007) contends that the younger generations are more susceptible to the effects of a significant social episode. This trend is obvious in the effects of the type of episodes that we are dealing with in the present paper. Following Pennebaker and Banasik (1997), Moaddel (2007) argues that the effect of any episode on young people below the age of 26 is more extensive than on older groups. Taking this into account, we argue that the young age cohort (e.g. below 26) is more likely to hold the old views about the episode of HIV/AIDS, because the young are more vulnerable to HIV/AIDS through sexual activities, and therefore, they might protect themselves from the spread of HIV/AIDS by holding conservative attitudes about sexuality. Thus, we need to evaluate the effects of event on different age groups in maintaining conservative attitudes about sexuality. In so doing, we coded ages below 26 as 1, and respondents aged 26 and above are coded 0. The hypothesis we will test by this measure is: The pervasive spread of HIV/AIDS results in more acceptance of older views about sexuality among younger cohort than the older cohort.The literature on political awareness suggests that higher levels of education implant an awareness pertaining the meaning of an episode (e.g. Zaller 1992 cited in Moaddel 2007). Likewise, the episode of HIV/AIDS influences more educated people with the negative meanings of sexual liberalism, because several major influential discourses blame this attitude for the spread of HIV/AIDS (see Kamwedo and Kamowa 1999). The educational levels are coded in World Values Surveys by lower, middle, and upper (1= Lower, 2=Middle, and 3=Upper). We propose the following hypothesis: The pervasive spread of HIV/AIDS results in more acceptance of older views about sexuality among people with more education than the people with lower education.The gender effects on values are obvious too. For HIV/AIDS has affected women the most, women are more likely to hold the view that sexual liberalism may cause huge problems for them. To assess this, we code female as 1, and male as 0. The hypothesis we would test by this measure is as follows: The pervasive spread of HIV/AIDS results in more acceptance of older views about sexuality among females than males.Dependent Variables The cultural inertia in attitudes pertaining
to sexuality and sexual behaviors is difficult to measure, for differential
cultural subjectivities are attached to these attitudes. As our earlier
discussion suggests, we need to measure cultural inertia by looking at
how attitudes about sexuality have been more or less static over time.
In so doing, we need to measure these attitudes.
In original dataset, the respondents were asked to rate their attitude in 1-10 point scales to assess whether homosexuality, divorce, abortion, prostitution, adultery, and sexual freedom are justifiable (1= not at all justifiable and 10= always justifiable). For the ease of interpretation, however, we reversed the codes so that higher values imply more conservatism, and as such, the stable higher scores over time attest to the existence of cultural inertia. Results HIV/AIDS: Is Significant a Social Episode? Figures 1 through 8 show that there is a positive association between HIV/AIDS and the perceptions on sexuality, meaning that people are more conservative when the rates for HIV/AIDS increase over time. Tables 1 and 2 also depict the positive correlations between HIV/AIDS and the conservative attitudes about sexual practices. Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3
Figure 4
Figure 5
Figure 6
Figure 7
Figure 8
Table 1.1
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). Recode of timeframe 1993-98 and 1999-04 = 1.00 Bring UP Tables
1.1 and 1.2 As One Image in New Window
Table 1.2
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). Recode of timeframe 1993-98 and 1999-04 = 1.00 Table 2.1
Recode of timeframe 1993-98 and 1999-04 = 2.00 Bring Up Tables 2.1 and 2.2 As One Table in New Window Table 2.2
Recode of timeframe 1993-98 and 1999-04 = 2.00 Table 3 presents results for regression
analyses. Over time, the effects of HIV/AIDS are supportive of our main
hypothesis that a social episode may bring inertia in cultural values.
Comparing the un-standardized betas for both panels with each other, clearly
the direction of the effects for HIV/AIDS in sustaining cultural inertia
towards conservative views about homosexuality, prostitution, abortion,
and divorce remain identical over time when the interaction terms are not
considered. This means that HIV/AIDS as a social episode greatly contributed
to the inertia of culture regarding attitudes about sexual behavior. This
supports our first hypothesis adequately.
Table 3.1
Table 3.2
Table 3.3
Table 3.4
As for as the interaction terms regarding attitudes toward homosexuality and prostitution, HIV/AIDS contributes to producing progressive views in peoples' minds if they are older males with no education. This pattern remains the same over time. When people are young and they have higher educational attainments, the increase of HIV/AIDS makes them more inclined towards conservative views, and this pattern is visible in both time-points. However, for the gender parameter, if the respondents are women, then HIV/AIDS increases conservative views over time. The findings for interaction terms for attitudes towards abortion indicate that the older males with no education perceive progressive views more than conservative views if HIV/AIDS increases over time. When people are young and they have higher educational attainments, the increase of HIV/AIDS makes them more inclined toward progressive views during 1999-2004, and this pattern is non-significant during 1993-1998. By contrast, when the respondents are females, HIV/AIDS increases the acceptance of progressive views toward abortion during 1999-2004, but this result is not significant for 1993-1998. Divorce is the only item where HIV/AIDS ceases to be significant in the final model when interaction terms were added for 1999-2004, although this remains significant as far as main effects concerned. When people are young, and they have better education than others, HIV/AIDS promotes the conservative views regarding divorce over time. The women of any society are more inclined toward conservative views about divorce when HIV/AIDS increases in 1999-2004, but this is not significant at the earlier time point. Age, Sex, and Education:
The effects of sex and education on the attitudes about prostitution, and divorce have not changed over time. It is noteworthy though, that the effects of young age have changed its direction somehow in the attitudes towards homosexuality, abortion, and divorce. For prostitution, the effects for young age, sex, and education remain the same over time. On homosexuality item, the young people seem to accept more conservative views over time, whereas the overall effect of the youngness as a result of HIV/AIDS on the attitudes about homosexuality remain the same, meaning that young people continue to uphold the same view about homosexuality over time. As estimated effects for interaction terms suggest, young people are more progressive if there is no HIV/AIDS at all in the country. By contrast, a 1% increase of HIV/AIDS leads young people to holding conservative views about homosexuality. The same conclusion can be drawn about prostitution. These support our hypotheses related to young age. For abortion, the results regarding the interaction between age and HIV/AIDS are somewhat different than expected. The effects of young age interacting with HIV/AIDS are non-significant for 1993-1998, while they are significant in 1999-2004. Clearly, the young people are more accepting of abortion when HIV/AIDS increases during this time. This is opposite to our hypothesis. When divorce is concerned, the interaction term between HIV/AIDS and age is significant for both time points. However, the simple effect of age when HIV/AIDS is zero suggests that the tendency of young people to accept divorce. The simple effects of age, sex, and education do not change the attitudes about homosexuality, prostitution, and divorce. For both homosexuality and divorce, if there is no HIV/AIDS, the young men with better education promote progressive views over time. In case of abortion, these findings are true for 1999-2004 as whole. Also, people with higher education accept progressive views when there is no HIV/AIDS in both panels. Discussion and Conclusion In general, the results suggest that HIV/AIDS as a social episode greatly contributed to the inertia of culture. Also, as the interaction effects suggest, the increase of HIV/AIDS promotes more conservative views about homosexuality and prostitution amongst more educated and young people. As for both abortion and divorce, the direction of the effects of age and education are somewhat opposite, when there is a spread of HIV/AIDS, to what was predicted. The reason lies in the fact that the young and educated people are more likely to understand the consequences of a particular episode, and therefore, the spread of HIV/AIDS could be limited if abortion and divorce are well accepted, because fewer number of HIV/AIDS infected babies would born to carry the risks incurred on society. As far as gender concerned, we note that females are more conservative on homosexuality and prostitution when HIV/AIDS spread, while they carry somewhat opposite views about abortion and divorce in the same situation upheld by HIV/AIDS. The latter part does not support what we hypothesized. However, what follows from here is that the prospective risk factor related to HIV/AIDS and the conservative views about abortion and divorce may put the women of the society at greater jeopardy; this could be the reason why women are progressive on these two parameters. We observe that the young and educated people become more progressive when a country does not have HIV/AIDS at all. The same is true for women too. As we conclude in the above assessment, HIV/AIDS and likely social episodes may lead to the inertia of some cultural attitudes about sexuality. The influences of age, sex, and gender are mixed when there is a massive spread of HIV/AIDS. The rationales for such findings involve the risk factor related to the social episode, and thus, in broader theoretical aspect, our hypotheses are not rejected. Even though some societies are expected to be more modern on some views, they may not be like that if there are social risks for a particular social episode. Therefore, social episodes are very important as social events. The future research on the topic may
explore the relationships between specific risks incurred by HIV/AIDS and
the inertia or change of cultural values. In so doing, larger datasets
representing more countries and timeframes would serve the research objective
as outlined here. Another suggestion regarding future research is to examine
whether a society could decelerate on cultural views.
Footnotes Return to Text (2)The reflexive
modernization as outlined by Beck (1992) relates to “unintended” consequences
of modernization itself. For example, nuclear accidents, global warming,
and ozone depletion.
References Alwin, D., Cohen, R. and Newcomb, T. 1991. Political Attitudes over the Life Span: The Bennington Women after Fifty Years. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press. Akosah, S. 2000. "Book Review of 'AIDS and Development
in Africa.'" EXPO TIMES.
Beck, U. 1992. Risk Society: Towards a New Modernity. London: Sage Publication. Beck, U. 1994. "The Reinvention of Politics: Towards a Theory of Reflexive Modernization." In Reflexive Modernization: Politics, Tradition and Aesthetics in the Modern Social Order, U. Beck and S. Lash (eds.), 70-84. Cambridge: Polity Press. Becker, Howard S. 1995. "The Power of Inertia." Qualitative Sociology 18(3): 301-309. Cameron, Edwin. 2001. "HIV/AIDS: Some Reflections on India and South Africa." Oxford University Commonwealth Law Journal 1(1): 35-36. Carrillo, Juan, and Denise Gromb. 2005. "Culture in Organization:
Inertia and Uniformity." Forthcoming in Journal of Law, Economics, and
Organization. Available at: http://www-rcf.usc.edu/
Colins, Randall. 1998. The Sociology of Philosophies: A Global Theory of Intellectual Change. Harvard University Press: Cambridge. Hope, Kempe Ronald, Sr. 1999. "The Socioeconomic Context of AIDS in Africa: A Review". In AIDS and Development in Africa, Kempe Ronald Hope Sr. (Ed.), 1-19. New York: The Haworth Press. Huntington, Samuel. 1996. The Clash of Civilizations and the Remarking of World Order. New York: Simon & Schuster Paperbacks. Inglehart, Ronald, and Christian Welzel. 2005. Modernization, Cultural Change, and Democracy. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kamwendo, Gregory, and Olex Kamowa. 1999. "HIV/AIDS and a Return to Traditional Cultural Practices in Malawi." In AIDS and Development in Africa, Kempe Ronald Hope Sr. (Ed.), 165-175. New York: The Haworth Press. Kaya, Hassan O. 1999. "Beyond the Statistics: HIV/AIDS As a Socioeconomic Epidemic in Africa." In AIDS and Development in Africa, Kempe Ronald Hope Sr. (Ed.), 37-45. New York: The Haworth Press. Moaddel, Mansoor. 2005. Islamic Modernism, Nationalism, and Fundamentalism: Episode and Discourse. University of Chicago Press: Chicago. Moaddel, Mansoor. 2007. "Events and Value Change: The Impacts of September 11, 2001 on the Worldviews of Egyptians and Moroccans." In Values and Perceptions of the Islamic and Middle Eastern Publics, Mansoor Moaddel (Ed.), 249-297. New York: Palgrave McMillan Moore, Barrinton, Jr. 1966. Social Change of Dictatorship and Democracy: Lord and Peasant in the Making of the Modern World. Bacon Press: Boston. Nisbett, Richard, and Dov Cohen. 1996. Culture of Honor: The Psychology of Violence in the South. Boulder: Westview Press. Pennebaker, James W., Dario Paez, and Bernard Rime. 1997. Collective Memory of Political Events: Social Psychological Perspectives. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pennebaker, James, and Becky Banasik. 1997. "On the Creation and Maintenance of Collective Memories." In Collective Memory of Political Events: Social Psychological Perspectives, Pennebaker, James W., Dario Paez, and Bernard Rime (Eds.), 3-19. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pick, David, and Kandy Dayaram. 2006. "Reflexive Judgement, Risk and Responses HIV/AIDS in Africa and Asia." Journal of Human Values 12(1): 55-64. Sewell, William H., Jr. 1996. "Historical Events As Transformations of Structures: Inventing Revolution at the Bastille." Theory and Society 25(6): 841-81. Ruel, Erin, and Richard Campbell. 2006. "Homophobia and HIV/AIDS: Attitude Change in the Face of an Epidemic". Social Forces 84(4):2167-2178. Ruxrungtham, Kiat, Tim Brown, Praphan Phanuphak. 2004. "HIV/AIDS in Asia." The Lancet 9428:69. Schneider, Mark. 2001. "Does Culture Have Inertia?" Newsletter of the Sociology of Culture: Section of the American Sociological Association 15(3): 1-5. Schuman, Howard, and Willard Rodgers. 2004. "Cohorts, Chronology, and Collective Memories." Public Opinion Quarterly 68(2): 217-54. Withnow, Robert. 2005. "Democratic Renewal and Cultural Inertia : Why Our Best Efforts Fall." Sociological Forum 20(3): 343-367. Withnow, Robert. 1989. Communities of Discourse: Ideology and Social Structure in the Reformation, the Enlightenment, and European Socialism. Cambridge: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates World Health Organization. 1999 thru 2006. Report on the Global AIDS Epidemic. Geneva: World Health Organization. Zaller, John. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass
Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Return to Sociation Today
Spring/Summer 2010
©2010 by the North Carolina Sociological Association |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||